Evaluation of the Effect of Peripheral Injection of Leptin on Spatial Memory
S Rasi, G Mohaddes, N Naghdi, M Ilkhani Pour, V Hajali
Keywords
leptin, morris water maze, rat, spatial memory
Citation
S Rasi, G Mohaddes, N Naghdi, M Ilkhani Pour, V Hajali. Evaluation of the Effect of Peripheral Injection of Leptin on Spatial Memory. The Internet Journal of Neurology. 2006 Volume 8 Number 2.
Abstract
Introduction
Leptin is a hormone that regulates body weight and energy homeostasis via its actions on specific hypothalamic nuclei (1). Leptin is the product of the obese (
The hippocampus has a well-documented role in spatial memory acquisition (8).
Farr and colleagues reported the role of leptin in learning and memory using an animal model. They found that mice navigated a maze better after they received leptin. Their research indicated that administration of leptin to mice improved retention of T-maze footshock avoidance and step down inhibitory avoidance (5). In addition, Oomura et al. showed a facilitation effect on learning and memory performance in passive avoidance and Morris water maze task after daily intravenous injection of leptin (50 ηg/kg) in rats (11). The other study suggested that leptin applied directly into the dentate gyrus; enhanced normal LTP at 1.0 ηM but inhibited LTP at lower and higher doses in the Morris water maze in urethane anesthetized rats (6). Just one experiment reported that leptin exhibit no effect on memory processes (11).
Since only a few studies investigated the involvement of systemic leptin in spatial memory formation and the subject is somehow controversial, we decided to assess the effect of different doses of intraperitoneal leptin on spatial memory in a Morris water maze task.
Material and Method
In order to assess the possibility of drug interference with animal sensory and motor coordination or the animal motivation, the capability of rats to escape to a visible platform was tested in this study. The trained rats were given four trials for visuo-motor coordination on the visible platform.
Results
During acquisition, the performance (traveled distance) of all groups improved with subsequent blocks of training. The difference between T1–T4 (block 1) and T5–T8 (block 2) was significant in each group (Fig. 1).
The one-way ANOVA of the escape latency of block 2 revealed significant differences between groups. Injection of 0.05 mg/kg (p= 0.050) and 0.1 mg/kg (p= 0.003) doses of leptin demonstrated better spatial learning than that of the saline treated animals. Animals treated with higher doses of leptin (0.25, 0.5 mg/kg) did not show any significant difference on water maze acquisition (Fig. 2).
Probe test data were compared between groups. One-way ANOVA of the time spent and distance traveled in the target quadrant revealed significant differences between groups. Animals treated with 0.1 mg/kg (p= 0.001) and 0.25 mg/kg (p= 0.029) of leptin significantly spent more time in the target quadrant than did the saline-treated group, indicating memory improvement in these animals. But Animals treated with 0.05 and 0.5 mg/kg of leptin show no significant difference in time spent in the target quadrant comparing with saline treated group (Fig. 3A). The average distance traveled in the target quadrant on the probe test showed that animals treated with doses of leptin 0.1 (p= 0.000), 0.25 (p= 0.017), 0.5 (p= 0.011) mg/kg demonstrated better spatial learning than that of the saline treated animals, as distance traveled in the target quadrant was significantly longer in this groups (Fig. 3B). Our data showed that leptin had no significant effect on swimming speed (data not shown).
Intraperitoneal leptin injection 30 min before visual trial (visible platform) also showed no significant difference in time spent to find the visible platform, compared to the control (Fig. 4).
Figure 1
Figure 2
Figure 3
Discussion
Our findings show that intraperitoneal injection of different doses of leptin improved spatial learning and memory. However, the improving effect of some doses was stronger than the improving effects of other doses.
Leptin enters areas throughout the brain by a system that is partially saturated at endogenous blood levels of leptin (14). Transporters for leptin carry it across the BBB to enter the interstitial fluid of the brain (3, 4) and Choroid plexus plays a key role in regulating leptin entry into the CSF under physiological conditions (15). Leptin outside the hypothalamus, where it plays a key role in energy expenditure and food intake, improves memory processing in the hippocampus (5). The hippocampus has been shown to be critically involved in learning and memory processes (16, 17, 18). Lesion of the CA1 subfield in rat spatial learning has been evaluated by the MWM (19).
Our data support previous results on leptin effects on different type of memory, such as in a water maze performance (6, 7, 20), T-maze footshock avoidance (5) and passive avoidance tasks (20). Oomura's rat study shows that leptin facilitated learning and memory in the Morris water-maze test, enhanced CA1 LTP maintenance, attenuated LTD, and led to increased CaMK II activity in the CA1 area (21). In addition, a close association between enhanced hippocampal LTP and facilitated learning and memory has been demonstrated (22, 23, 24, 25, 26 , 27). Farr and colleagues assessed the role of leptin in memory processing using two different avoidance paradigms. Their results indicate the leptin improves memory processing for T-maze footshock avoidance in SAM-P8 male mice (5).
Recent studies investigated hippocampal long-term potentiation (LTP) and long-term depression (LTD), and the spatial-memory function in two leptin receptor-deficient rodents (Zucker rats and db/db mice). In brain slices, the CA1 hippocampal region of both strains showed impairments of LTP and LTD; leptin did not improve these impairments in either strain (7). Another experiment determined the effects of leptin, 0.0, 1.0, 100 nM, 1, and 10ηM, applied directly into the dentate gyrus, on LTP in medial perforant path dentate granule cell synapses in male Harlan Sprague-Dawley rats. Its findings suggest that leptin enhances normal LTP at 1.0ηM but inhibits LTP at lower and higher doses (6).
In the present behavioral study intraperitoneal injection of leptin improved spatial learning and memory. However, the improving effect of dose (0.1mg/kg) was stronger than the improving effects of other doses. Leptin with lower (0.05 mg/kg) and also higher doses (0.25, 0.5 mg/kg) had weaker effects on water maze task, which indicates there is an optimal dose for memory.
Recent behavioral and LTP experiments has also demonstrate that leptin shows an inverted-U dose related function in terms of its effects on learning and memory and LTP. Similar inverted-U-shaped nature on the dose–response curve for leptin has been reported in memory processing in mice performing step down passive avoidance test (5), and in LTP in the dentate gyrus of anesthetized rats (6), when leptin is administered directly into the CA1 region and the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus, respectively.
Collectively, it is possible that the 0.05 mg/kg dose of leptin is not enough to trigger cellular effects. Also, the weak effect of higher doses of leptin on spatial learning and memory probably could be attributed to the limiting effect of choroids epithelium in transferring of hormone to cerebrospinal fluid. Because leptin transport across the blood-CSF barrier is fully saturated at higher leptin physiological plasma concentrations, it is possible that the choroid epithelium acts as a rate-limiting step to prevent increases in CSF leptin concentrations (15).
On the other hand, it is possible that higher doses of leptin trigger other types of receptors or other intracellular signaling pathways. For example, it may be related to the effect of leptin on internalization of AMPA receptors in hippocampal CA1 neurons (28). AMPA receptor-mediated synaptic transmission in the hippocampus is critical for encoding and consolidation of spatial (29), aversive (30) and recognition memory (31, 32). Maybe, leptin inhibits hippocampal cells through AMPA receptor down-regulation. It had also shown that leptin inhibits rat hippocampal neurons by increasing a K+ conductance (33).
Other findings show that leptin; at concentrations comparable with those circulating in the plasma (34) can modulate hippocampal synaptic plasticity, by conversion of STP into LTP. A key process underlying this effect is the enhancement of NMDA responses; a process not only requiring activation of PI 3-kinase, but also MAPK and Src tyrosine kinases. A crucial intracellular process regulating NMDA receptor function is phosphorylation (35), and both serine– threonine and tyrosine phosphorylation regulate NMDA receptor function. In particular, Src tyrosine kinases can directly phosphorylate NMDA receptor NR2A (36) and NR2B (37) subunits. Functionally this may be important in hippocampal synaptic plasticity because it has been hypothesized that during LTP induction, Src is rapidly activated leading to enhanced NMDA receptor function (38).
Taken together, our results and previous studies indicate that the same peptide could possibly have different modulator post synaptic effects in different hippocampal synapses dependent upon different types of post synaptic receptors (5, 6).
In summary, we found that leptin improves memory and it shows its highest effect with medium doses.