Mood, Weight, and Physical Activity Among Obese Individuals Enrolled In a Long-term Weight-loss Program: Trajectories and Associations with Gender
S Langer, A Flood, E Welsh, R Levy, M Jaeb, P Laqua, A Hotop, N Mitchell, R Jeffery
Keywords
distress, gender, mood, physical activity, vigor, weight-loss
Citation
S Langer, A Flood, E Welsh, R Levy, M Jaeb, P Laqua, A Hotop, N Mitchell, R Jeffery. Mood, Weight, and Physical Activity Among Obese Individuals Enrolled In a Long-term Weight-loss Program: Trajectories and Associations with Gender. The Internet Journal of Mental Health. 2008 Volume 6 Number 1.
Abstract
Objective: To examine the trajectories of mood, weight and physical activity, and associations between mood, weight, and gender, among 213 obese individuals.Methods: Prospective, longitudinal design. Assessments at baseline and 6, 12, and 18 months of Profile of Mood States, Paffenbarger Physical Activity Questionnaire, and weight.Results: Total mood disturbance decreased from baseline to 6 months, with no change thereafter. Weight decreased from baseline to 6 to 12 months, and increased from 12 to 18 months. Physical activity increased from baseline to 6 months, and 12 to 18 months. Increased physical activity predicted greater vigor and less fatigue over time. Females high in distress at 6 months lost less weight than females low in distress and at 18 months gained more weight than those low in distress. There were no such associations among males.Conclusion: The trajectories of mood, weight and physical activity were synchronous only in the short-term. Distress monitoring, targeted to females who relapse, may be warranted.
Supported by grant R01 DK064596 from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Diseases, awarded to Dr. Jeffery.
Introduction
Much attention has been paid to the relationships among mood, eating behavior, and weight ( 1 ). Empirical investigations designed to address this topic have employed disparate methodologies and designs: cross-sectional ( 2 , 3 ), longitudinal ( 1 , 4 ), and experimental, whereby mood states are manipulated and resultant food consumption is examined ( 5 , 6 , 7 ), or food consumption is manipulated and resultant mood states are examined ( 8 ). Findings are complex, indicating non-linear and bi-directional relationships. For example, in laboratory-based studies, negative mood states have been associated with both decreased ( 9 ) and increased food consumption ( 10 ). The latter is referred to as the stress-eating paradox, given the typical pattern of appetite suppression and decreased food intake associated with autonomic activation consequent to stress.
In epidemiologic investigations, examination of the relationship between mood and weight has tended to focus specifically on the relationship between mood disorders and obesity. Some studies report a positive relationship between mood disorders and overweight or obesity ( 2 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 , 15 ), whereas others report an inverse relationship ( 16 , 17 ), or no relationship ( 18 ). For still others, a positive relationship occurs only among females ( 3 , 19 , 20 ), and an inverse relationship occurs among males ( 19 , 21 ).
It would be remiss to mention such gender differences without considering restrained eating, defined as “the volitional effort to restrict food intake to control body weight” ( 22 , p. 16 ). Females are more likely to engage in this behavior than are males ( 23 , 24 , 25 ). They are also more likely to engage in emotional eating, increased consumption in response to negative emotional experience ( 23 , 26 ). The association between negative affect and food intake differs for restrained and unrestrained eaters. Distress increases food consumption among restrained eaters but not unrestrained eaters. This association has been demonstrated in both experimental ( 5 , 27 ) and naturalistic settings ( 28 ). The relationship between restrained eating and weight control, moreover, differs by gender, such that restraint is detrimental for females but beneficial for males ( 24 ).
To summarize, results from a large number of investigations, while variant in methodology, suggest important relationships between mood, eating behavior and weight. However, findings are inconsistent and, in some cases, differ by gender. In addition, most studies involving obese populations have focused on mood disorders (e.g., clinical levels of depression or anxiety). Furthermore, few studies have afforded examination of associations between mood and weight within the context of formalized weight-loss programs. An early review indicated deleterious emotional consequences of weight-loss programs ( 29 ), whereas more recent studies indicated either no association ( 30 ) or benefit, such as increased vitality ( 31 , 32 ), decreased anxiety, decreased depression, and decreased fatigue ( 31 ). The present investigation represents an observational, longitudinal analysis of mood and weight among obese individuals enrolled in a long-term behavioral weight-loss program (i.e., individuals instructed to restrain their eating behavior and increase their energy expenditure through physical activity).
Physical activity, like ingestion, is known to affect mood. Results from a wide range of studies, including randomized controlled trials, suggest that exercise improves mood, even after just single bouts of activity ( 33 , 34 , 35 ). Conversely, exercise withdrawal is associated with mood decrements and fatigue ( 36 ). In their theoretical model, Baker and Brownell ( 37 ) posit relationships between physical activity, psychological mechanisms, and weight control. Exercise is associated with mood improvement, which in turn is associated with increased motivation and commitment. Such commitment leads to greater dietary compliance and exercise adherence, which in turn results in greater weight-control success. Conversely, limited physical activity and mood decrements result in weight-control failure.
The aims of the present investigation were three-fold. First, we sought to examine the trajectories of mood, weight and physical activity over time, as individuals attempted to lose weight. Synchronous trajectories were expected (e.g., mood improvement concurrent with weight-loss and increased physical activity). Second, we sought to examine the relationship between distress and weight-change as a function of gender. In line with past research ( 3 , 19 , 20 ), the relationship between distress and weight-change was expected to be stronger for females as compared to males. To note,
Methods
Design and Participants
Participants were recruited from the Minneapolis community in two waves to the Lose It Forever (LIFE) study - a randomized controlled trial designed to test the efficacy of different combinations of behavioral (diet and exercise) prescriptions for weight-loss and then weight-maintenance. In the trial, participants were randomly assigned to receive either standard behavioral therapy or a maintenance-tailored treatment emphasizing variety and adaptation to change. Repeated assessments occurred at baseline, 6 months, 12 months, and 18 months. We focus here on the observational, prospective, longitudinal aspect of the study, collapsing across treatment condition. Primary results of the trial, with weight-loss and weight-maintenance as outcomes, are reported elsewhere ( 39 ) and beyond the scope of the present manuscript.
Recruitment methods included mass media advertising and, to attract men and minorities, specialized advertising targeted to those populations. Eligible participants were at least 18 years old, with a BMI of 30-39. Exclusion criteria were: current use of weight-loss medications or participation in another organized weight loss program; history or presence of cancer, cardiovascular disease, diabetes, chronic fatigue, arthritis, fibromyalgia, or stroke; inability to walk at least 10 minutes without stopping; current pharmacologic or behavioral treatment for a major psychological disorder; and current use of a synthroid drug. In addition, females were excluded if they were pregnant, < 6 months postpartum, breastfeeding, or planning to become pregnant in the ensuing 30 months.
Procedures
Study procedures were IRB-approved, and informed consent was obtained from all participants. Clinic-measured assessments included weight, height and waist/hip ratio. Weight and height were measured in light clothing, without shoes, using a wall-mounted stadiometer in centimeters with a horizontal measuring block, and a Tanita BWB 800 digital scale, read in kilograms (Tanita corp., Arlington Heights, IL). Participants also completed a battery of pencil-and-paper measures, including demographic items, the Profile of Mood States (POMS), and the Paffenbarger Activity Questionnaire.
The POMS is a measure of affect or mood ( 40 ). Respondents rate the extent to which they experienced feelings such as
The Paffenbarger Activity Questionnaire is a well-known measure of physical activity ( 42 ). In this questionnaire, respondents are asked to report the number of stairs climbed, blocks walked, and the type and duration of all leisure-time physical activities during the past week. Responses are scored to provide an estimate of kilocalories expended per week in overall leisure time activity, and in activities of light (5 kcal/min), mixed (7.5 kcal/min), and high (10 kcal/min) intensity. We focus here on the former, average weekly caloric expenditure. Test-retest reliability of the scale was demonstrated in several studies spanning disparate samples ( 43 , 44 , 45 ). Validity was demonstrated by appropriate relationships with BMI ( 46 ), body fat ( 43 ), maximum oxygen consumption ( 43 , 44 ), and self-reported physical activity and sweating ( 46 ).
Statistical Analyses
Descriptive and inferential analyses were performed using SPSS 13.0 and SAS 8.1. A series of linear mixed model regression analyses were conducted to examine the effects of time on several dependent variables: POMS TMD, each of the POMS subscales, weight, and weekly average energy expenditure in kilocalories. Mixed models adjust for the correlation of repeated measures over time and allow for missing data. In all models, time was treated as a fixed effect, with baseline serving as the referent. Study wave, gender and treatment condition were also included in the models as fixed effects, as was a time x treatment condition interaction for the POMS models.
Additional analyses afforded examination of physical activity (energy expenditure) as a predictor of mood over time (POMS TMD and each of the subscales). Two variables were created to indicate both between- and within-person variability in expenditure. Between-person variability can be thought of as simple mean differences in energy expenditure between participants. Within-person variability can be thought of as the time-specific deviation from an individual’s own mean energy expenditure. These two variables were included in a mixed linear regression model as time-varying covariates, along with wave, gender, and treatment condition.
For all of the models detailed above, the covariance matrix was unstructured. This is a conservative approach because it does not assume that correlation within a subject (in this case, a subject’s mood, weight or energy expenditure) is fixed and constant over time. Three females who became pregnant during the course of the study were excluded from analyses.
Finally, linear regression analysis was used to examine the relationship between mood, weight-change, and gender. Three regressions were run for each of three weight- change dependent variables: baseline to 6-month weight-change, 6-month to 12-month weight-change, and 12-month to 18-month weight-change. This was done separately for males and females. The change variable was calculated by subtracting weight at a given time point from weight at the subsequent time point, with positive values indicating gain and negative values indicating loss. For each analysis, POMS TMD scores were entered as a predictor of weight-change, treated as a dichotomy based on a median split (0 = low distress, 1 = high distress). For each regression, POMS TMD at the later of the two time points was used, e.g., 6 months for the analysis involving baseline to 6-month weight change.
Results
Table 1 displays baseline demographic and clinical characteristics of a) the total sample (n = 213) and b) the “completer” subsample of those still participating at 18 months (n = 159). Looking at the enrolled/ total sample, participants were, on average, 49 years old, 53% female, and 32% non-Caucasian. By self-report, 77% were college-educated. Mean baseline weight and BMI values were 103 kg and 35 kg/m 2 , respectively. Over one-half of participants had previously participated in an organized weight-loss program. Descriptively, the “completer” subsample did not differ appreciably from the enrolled sample.
Figure 1
Trajectories of Mood, Weight, and Physical Activity
Means and standard errors for TMD, weight, and energy expenditure (as a function of time) are displayed in Figures 1a-c, respectively. As seen in the top panel of Figure 1, POMS TMD significantly decreased from baseline to 6 months (beta = -0.31,
Figure 1a-c. Profile of Mood States Total Mood Disturbance (POMS TMD, top panel), Weight (middle panel), and Weekly Average Caloric Expenditure (bottom panel) as a Function of Time, M (SE). Values reflect all available data per time point (pairwise deletion).
Means and standard errors for each of the POMS subscales are displayed in Figure 2. The patterns of change over time were similar for tension-anxiety, anger-hostility, fatigue-inertia and confusion-bewilderment. Negative affect significantly decreased from baseline to 6 months (beta = -0.10,
Physical Activity as a Predictor of Mood
Additional analyses afforded examination of energy expenditure as a predictor of mood over time. Increased energy expenditure was associated with greater vigor (beta = 0.01,
Mood, Weight-change and Gender
Discussion
The first aim of this study was simply to examine the trajectories of mood, weight and physical activity over time. On average, weight decreased from baseline to 6 months, and from 6 to 12 months, but
Weight-loss was associated with improved mood only during the early period of rapid change. Weight decreased from baseline to 6 months, as did TMD. However, a further reduction in negative affect was not seen as weight continued to drop, nor was an increase seen as weight was regained. Perhaps the weight-loss itself became less reinforcing over time, or the secondary benefits of weight-loss such as positive attention and praise diminished over time. This is in line with theoretical work on motivation ( 47 ). As goals are initially met (e.g., as weight is first lost), motivation strengthens. However, motivation weakens with repeated exposure to the goal.
The relationship between distress and exercise was inverse (as expected), but only in the short-term. Exercise increased from baseline to 6 months, just as TMD decreased. This is in line with past research on the emotional benefits of physical activity ( 33 ). Interestingly, we did not see a concomitant improvement in mood as energy expenditure increased from 12 to 18 months. Moreover, while energy expenditure was associated with greater vigor and less fatigue over time, it was not associated with overall distress, sadness, anxiety or anger. This finding is commensurate with experimental studies designed to examine the effects of exercise on mood utilizing the POMS ( 48 ). It could be that the effects of physical activity on mood are more specific to energy and activation as opposed to discrete emotional states such as sadness or anger. Given the non-experimental nature of our study, we do not know whether positive mood states fostered better dedication to an exercise program or if physical activity led to increased vitality. According to Baker and Brownell ( 37 ), exercise enhances mood, which in turn increases commitment and adherence.
Consistent with past research ( 3 , 20 ), the relationship between mood and weight was stronger for females than for males. Females high in emotional distress at 6 months had lost less weight since the start of the study than females low in emotional distress at 6 months. Similarly, females high in emotional distress at 18 months had lost less weight since 12 months as compared to females low in emotional distress at 18 months. In fact, on average, females high in distress at 18 months had
It is important to consider who was not represented in the study, either by exclusion or drop-out over time. Persons with a chronic disease (e.g., diabetes) were excluded from participation. This may have resulted in a sample with lower baseline levels of distress. While the enrolled/ total sample did not differ appreciably from the subsample of participants who completed the study, there were fewer African Americans. This is fitting with research indicating greater obesity and less weight-loss success among African Americans ( 51 , 52 , 53 ).
There are multiple facets of emotion and the present investigation sought to examine just one, that of self-reported emotional experience. It would be interesting, in future research, to examine an observable indicator of emotion among weight- loss intervention participants, such as the emotional valence of words uttered during group-format treatment sessions. In addition, self-reported assessments in our study were few and far between. Diary studies, or the use of ecological momentary assessment methods, may prove exceedingly informative. An extended follow-up is also warranted.
Finally, future research should consider individual differences and the potential moderating role of such differences. Examples include baseline or concurrent differences in body image, real/ideal body weight synchrony or lack thereof, and stress reactivity. With respect to the latter, research indicates associations between high stress reactivity, increased cortisol, increased intake of calorically dense food, and weight, specifically, central adiposity ( 54 , 55 , 56 ). With respect to cognitions regarding weight, weight
Limitations aside, findings from this study extend the literatures on weight-loss and weight-maintenance by examining the trajectories of mood, weight, and physical activity among participants enrolled in a long-term behavioral weight-loss intervention. Trajectories were synchronous only in the short-term, offering implications for interventions designed to enhance mood within the context of formal weight-loss programs, not necessarily at the outset (as weight is lost and mood improves) but perhaps mid-way or later on in the process, as individuals begin to lapse. Females in particular should be monitored for distress, especially if their weight-loss efforts have been unsuccessful. Lastly, exercise is recommended, not just for weight-loss but also for psychosocial functioning, specifically, increased vigor and decreased fatigue.
Acknowledgements
Special thanks to all of the participants and Emily Finch.