ISPUB.com / IJMH/5/1/12151
  • Author/Editor Login
  • Registration
  • Facebook
  • Google Plus

ISPUB.com

Internet
Scientific
Publications

  • Home
  • Journals
  • Latest Articles
  • Disclaimers
  • Article Submissions
  • Contact
  • Help
  • The Internet Journal of Mental Health
  • Volume 5
  • Number 1

Original Article

A Set of Meta-Analytic Studies on the Factors Associated with Disordered Eating

E Vince, I Walker

Keywords

anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, eating disorders, meta analysis

Citation

E Vince, I Walker. A Set of Meta-Analytic Studies on the Factors Associated with Disordered Eating. The Internet Journal of Mental Health. 2007 Volume 5 Number 1.

Abstract


Objective: To consolidate knowledge from research on associates of disordered eating to guide future research efforts, asking “which factors are associated with the presence of disturbed eating/ anorexia/ bulimia?”
Method: We reviewed 232 studies, comprising 87,878 participants, through 74 individual meta-analyses under 12 associative factor category headings.
Results: Race had no association (r = .02), whilst anxiety (r = .47) and depression (r = .39) were modestly associated with disordered eating. Women with eating disorders were more likely than controls to have experienced abuse (r = .19), personality disorders (r = .26), an increased tendency to self-harm (r = .37), various personality traits such as perfectionism (r = .30) and hostility (r = .40), and engage in more exercise (r = .13). A higher incidence of stressful life events were reported by women with bulimia (r = .23) but not by women with other eating disorders.
Conclusions: Although disturbed eating, anorexia and bulimia show similar associations with various factors, they also show some disorder-specific relationships, highlighting overlapping and unique aspects of the disorders. Suggestions to benefit further research are proposed based on these differences.

 

The work was conducted at the Department of Psychology, University of Bath, UK

Introduction

In the quest to understand the causes and development of eating disorders, many risk factors and associates have been proposed and investigated, and the research corpus has become incredibly large. A small proportion of this research has been longitudinal/prospective in design and so has firmly established certain factors (e.g., perfectionism, as contributory causes of eating disorders). These longitudinal studies have recently been subjected to a comprehensive meta-analysis ( 1 ) which provided perspective on current knowledge about eating disorder causal factors. A limitation to this meta-analysis, however, was the exclusion of studies with correlational and retrospective data. These data still have the potential to inform the field if treated appropriately. Specifically, although research on associates cannot reliably establish causes and effects, it can still inform us about factors associated with eating behaviours, and so remains of value to the researcher interested in eating disorders, especially given the large number of participants who have taken part in this research.

To dismiss these studies because their methodology does not allow conclusive statements about the aetiology of eating disorders, is premature and unnecessary. However, at the same time we feel a point has been reached where correlational research is in such abundance that there is a need to consolidate it to reveal the state of our knowledge about associates of eating disorders as a way of guiding future research efforts. This is particularly pressing given that the meta-analysis by Stice ( 1 ) did not cover a range of potential associate factors suggesting they have not yet been investigated via longitudinal methods (e.g., autonomy, exercise, and obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD)). Factors that seem most promising from this objective integration of the literature might then be studied using other and more powerful methods to investigate more accurately the issue of cause and effect.

Method

There are various ways to synthesize the data provided by reviewing a body of literature, and perhaps the most heralded is to use meta-analytic methods. To provide the broadest possible review of how eating behaviours and attitudes are related to the various associate factors under investigation, the primary research question was “which factors are associated with the presence of disturbed eating (defined as any disturbance, clinical or sub-clinical, measured with a clinical diagnosis or, in non-clinical populations, with a scale such as the Eating Disorder Inventory)?” Secondary research questions focused the review on more specific diagnoses: “which factors are associated with anorexia nervosa?” and “which factors are associated with bulimia nervosa?” where studies were only included if participants had received a clinical diagnosis. We maintained this classic diagnostic line of distinguishing between anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa as there is little data, especially from the older studies, on factors associated with the sub-types of these disorders, or the presence of specific behaviours such as purging.

Several a priori decisions were made regarding the type of data and studies that would be included in the review. First, since so many measures of eating disorders have been employed in the research, it was decided that data collected from a wide range of measures would be used in the meta-analysis to gain the broadest picture of which aspects may be associated with disordered eating. For example, indices of weight concern, eating psychopathology, body image distortion, and body dissatisfaction, were considered eligible outcome measures, as well as popular instruments like the Eating Disorders Inventory (EDI ( 2 )) and the Eating Attitudes Test (EAT ( 3 )). Second, it was decided to only use studies written in English, published between 1987 and 2003 (there is a marked drop in the number of published studies before 1987, showing that it was the beginning of the rapid growth of eating disorder research) and that were non-longitudinal in nature (i.e., correlational, case-control, cross-sectional). Third, only data from female participants aged 13 or over were included. Finally, studies were only included if they contained at least one of the following comparisons: (a) levels of assessed factors in an eating-disordered group and a non-eating-disordered group; (b) a group with a putative associate factor compared to a group without that factor, assessing eating disorder prevalence in each group; and (c) correlations between measures of disordered eating and the putative associate factor. Excluded comparisons were: (a) an eating-disordered group compared to another eating-disordered group (e.g., a bulimic group with the putative associate factor compared to a bulimic group without it); and (b) correlations within an all-eating-disordered group. Such comparisons were inappropriate for this review as they did not allow us to assess the extent to which a factor is associated with the prevalence of disordered eating. All studies relating to eating disorder prevention, treatment, or measurement development were excluded, as were any studies that did not report statistics suitable for meta-analysis.

Search strategy

Articles that met the inclusion criteria outlined above were identified as follows: 1) a computer search of PsycINFO, BIDS, Ingenta, ScienceDirect (medicine, psychology, and social science categories), and Web of Science electronic publication databases was conducted for the relevant years, using the search terms “eating disorders” “anorexia nervosa”, “bulimia nervosa” “risk factors” and where appropriate excluding “prospective” and “longitudinal” with the resultant studies assessed for relevance by the researcher and categorised under 12 headings (e.g., “abuse”, “exercise”, and “race”); 2) each category of factor was then subjected to a further search utilising category-specific search terms (e.g., search terms relating to the abuse category included “abuse”, “sex abuse”, “physical abuse”, “emotional abuse”, and “childhood sexual abuse”); 3) the References sections of all the articles were examined; 4) all articles in the authors' possession were checked for relevance and their References sections also examined; 5) in attempting to locate articles, papers were requested from authors, which often led them to send other articles that they had written.

Of the several thousand articles retrieved through this strategy, 700 were deemed appropriate for further scrutiny, of which 232 were included in the review. This demonstrates just how large the field of eating disorder research has become, and in particular, how much correlational data has been generated. We feel this amply justifies this review, as synthesizing this large body of research is likely the most appropriate way to derive concrete conclusions from such a vast volume of data.

Procedure

Data were extracted by the principal author (EV), in line with the coding sheet developed for this review. In the event of uncertainty regarding the data, the second author independently extracted the data and results were compared. Any inconsistencies were discussed until an agreement was reached. As long as the data met the criteria outlined above, the study was included.

The meta-analyses were conducted using Schwarzer's ( 4 ) freely available software Meta, which utilises the procedures of Hunter, Schmidt and Jackson ( 5 ) and Hedges and Olkin ( 6 ). All analyses used a random-effects model with effect sizes weighted for sample size and corrected with Fisher's z-transformation ( 7 ). The analyses all followed a similar format. Each began with an analysis investigating whether the category in question (e.g., substance use) was related to a general eating disturbance, by including all measures of substance use and all levels of eating disturbance (specific and general forms, sub-clinical, and non-clinical). In calculating the overall relationship, three tests of effect-size homogeneity were conducted: residual standard deviation is smaller than a quarter of the sampling error variance (Sp, SD< ¼), observed variance is accounted for by at least 75% of the sampling error (Se), and a non-significant chi-square for systematic variation. If the results of at least two of these tests showed that the effect sizes were homogeneous then the overall meta-analysis effect size could simply be interpreted as a measure of association. However, when the effect sizes showed heterogeneity, an inductive moderator search ( 4 ) was conducted to search for study characteristics that could explain the variance in effect sizes. This involved a hierarchical cluster analysis to identify clusters of studies with similar effect sizes, then, using the coding sheet, identifying various study features to explain cluster membership. Additionally we distinguish between mixed heterogeneity and varied heterogeneity. The former is when the effect sizes differ in direction as well as magnitude and so show fundamental disagreement in the literature, whilst the latter agree on the direction of the association and differ only in magnitude.

When the data allowed, further analyses were conducted to look at the relationships between each associate factor and the two clinical forms of eating disorder, anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN). In some cases it was also appropriate to sub-divide the overall factor – for example, in the substance use analysis, separate analyses for the different types of substance use were conducted, first with general eating disturbance, and then with anorexia and bulimia where possible, providing a more detailed picture of any associations.

Effect sizes

As the emphasis of this paper is the co-occurrence of associate factors and eating disorders, r, with its connotations of association, is the natural choice of effect size measure. It also has the advantage that it is by far the most widely understood measure of effect size ( 8,9 ), and can be readily converted to other effect-size measures such as d. Cohen's ( 10 ) rough guide to what constitute small, medium and large effect sizes for r (r > 0.10, r > 0.30 and r > 0.50, respectively) are applied throughout the analyses to provide a loose, but useful scale of magnitude to guide interpretation.

Results

For each meta-analysis, number of studies (k) and number of participants (N) are reported alongside the effect size, and its corresponding z and p measures to assess statistical significance. For each of the meta-analyses the “fail-safe N” statistic, Nfs is also reported to address the “file drawer problem” ( 11 ), in which it is assumed that owing to submission and publication biases, for every published study showing a relationship between two variables, there is an unknown number of unpublished studies showing no relationship between the same two variables. Usually, the value of Nfs reported in a meta-analysis is the number of unpublished studies that would need to exist to turn a significant effect size into a non-significant one. Here we take a more conservative approach and present instead Nfs|.10| , which is the number of unpublished studies that would have to exist to reduce the obtained effect size to an effect size of r = .10, which, is the smallest effect size generally held to be of interest. We interpreted Nfs|.10| such that if Nfs|.10| for a given significant effect was equal to or higher than k, the result was assumed almost certainly to indicate a robust effect, if Nfs|.10| was less than k then the result was classed as not robust. This rule-of-thumb was chosen as it was unlikely that the same number of studies again for any given analysis existed due to the comprehensive literature search. This approach was used rather than using funnel plots as it provides an actual value (rather than a visual display) that can easily be compared across studies as to whether the effect found is robust or not in terms of the effect of potential unpublished studies.

Due to the retrospective nature of the factors in this series of meta-analyses, we have been carefully neutral about using the term “associate factors” rather than “risk factors”. Although it is the assumption of much work to date that many of the factors are probably causes of the eating disorders, many of these risk factors are also plausibly consequences of the eating disorder rather than antecedents. With this meta-analysis providing clear evidence of which factors have definite relationships with eating disorders, it is for future research to further explore which are causes and which are consequences of the eating problems.

The meta-statistics for each analysis can be found in Table 1 below, whereas the details of the studies included in the analysis can be found in Appendix 1.

Figure 1
Table 1: Statistical information for the series of meta-analyses.

Abuse

Based on our analysis, women who have suffered abuse are more likely to display eating disturbances. Abuse, in general, was related to eating disturbance with a small effect size (r = .19), as was sexual abuse (r = .20). While physical abuse was associated with eating disturbance (r = .12), the result was not robust in terms of the fail safe N. More research would remedy this issue and would also determine whether the mechanisms and links to eating disturbance for physical abuse differ from those of sexual abuse. A disorder-specific analysis demonstrated a small-medium association (r = .26) between BN and general abuse.

Sexuality

While there was a small association (r = .10) between sexuality and disturbed eating, the result was not robust in terms of the fail safe N value and showed mixed effect sizes. The inductive moderator search showed that culture and age may explain the variance in effect sizes. However, due to the unstable meta-analytical result, the evidence on the relationship between sexuality and eating disturbance is inconclusive, and further work is needed to explore the potential moderators further.

Self-harm

Based on our analyses, women with disturbed eating behaviour exhibited a higher level of self-harm behaviours than do non-eating disturbed women. To highlight, disturbed eating showed a modest relationship (r = .37) with self-harm behaviours. Although effect sizes were all positive, variation was seen between them, which may be a result of the assessment measures used: the first cluster of studies (k = 2, weighted mean r = .32), used self-harm-specific assessment measures, whereas a unique study ( 12 , r = .61) used only selected sub-scales from a personality assessment measure.

Relationship with Primary Care Giver

Two main concepts were combined and investigated, both assessing the quality of the fundamental relationship with the primary caregiver: attachment style and perceived parental bonding. The overall analysis found a moderate association (r = .30) between general eating disturbance and poor caregiver relationship, but the effect sizes varied. Analyses of AN and BN both demonstrated that eating-disordered women reported more maladaptive relationships with their caregivers (r = .26, r = .40 respectively). However, both analyses again showed heterogeneity in effect sizes; the moderator search revealed no meaningful suggestions in the AN analysis, while in the BN analysis, much larger effect sizes were found in the cluster containing the most recent studies (mean publication year of 2001, mean weighted r = .54, versus 1992, mean weighted r =.23). This finding may reflect either a true higher level of dysfunction, or changes in reporting behaviour. Overall, these analyses indicate that women displaying eating disordered behaviours, especially the clinical variants of AN and BN, are more likely than control women to report dysfunctional relationships with their caregivers.

Family environment

Analyses of family environment were mixed in result, but overall indicate that women displaying disordered eating, especially AN and BN, report more family dysfunction than control groups. There was a small association (r = .13) between eating disturbance and family environment, but this was not robust and mixed effect sizes were seen. A cluster analysis indicated that this mixed effect may be explained by age: the Waller et al. ( 13 ) study found a unique negative effect (N = 78, r = – .24, mean age 26) using a sample that was significantly older than in the other studies (N =26, r = .19, mean age 20). However, we recommend further research on the influences of participant age on this factor. Specific analyses for AN and BN found small effects of family environment (r = .09, r = .20 respectively). Although the BN analysis was robust and the effect sizes were all similar, the AN analysis was non-robust with mixed effect sizes. Again age was identified as a probable moderator, with removal of the Waller et al's older sample ( 13 ) strengthening the association.

Stress

Stress was not associated with disturbed eating (r = .03), albeit with heterogeneous effect sizes. Exploration of this heterogeneity showed that negative effect sizes were found in studies using only non-clinical samples and no association was found in studies simply assessing number of life events as opposed to the perceived severity of the stress. A disorder-specific analysis found a small association between BN and stress, but again with varied heterogeneity in the effect sizes.

Irrational cognitions

An overall analysis found an association (r = .40) between general eating disturbance and irrational cognitions (a commonly used term to encompass negative self-beliefs/core beliefs or dysfunctional cognitions and underlying assumptions regarding eating, weight and body shape seen in people with eating disorders). Effect sizes ranged from a trivial r = .05 ( 14 ) to an impressive r = .89 ( 15 ), with 14 out of the 17 studies reporting an effect size of .28 or greater. Exploration of the heterogeneity found that studies using eating-specific measures of irrational cognitions found the strongest associations. A stratification analysis based on measurement scale was conducted to explore this further. Although all the eating-specific measures (Dysfunctional Attitudes Scale [DAS]; Irrational Beliefs Scale [IBS]; Young's Schema Questionnaire [YSQ]; Eating Disorder Belief Questionnaire [EDBQ]) found associations between irrational cognitions and disturbed eating, only the DAS resulted in homogeneous effect sizes. Although the DAS's conceptualisation of dysfunctional attitudes may differ from that of the beliefs assessed in the other three measures, our finding does imply that measurement scale was not solely responsible for the initial effect size heterogeneity. The analyses for the specific eating disorders of AN and BN produced results both similar to each other and to the overall analysis (r = .62, r = .61 respectively) with varied effect sizes which were not readily explicable.

Overall, the analyses demonstrated that women with disturbed eating, and AN and BN specifically, show higher levels of irrational cognitions than do women with no eating problems, although there is substantial variance in the strength of this finding. When cognitions are assessed by the DAS, the relationship appears more stable and robust than when measured with other relevant measures. However, we suggest more work is needed to further explore possible reasons for the range of effect sizes.

Exercise

Exercise level showed a small association with disturbed eating (r = .13). However, this result was not robust and showed mixed effect sizes, with 14 studies suggesting higher levels of exercise are found in those who have disturbed eating, and 12 studies suggesting exercise levels are marginally higher in control women. Exploration of the heterogeneity found that the underlying research questions posed may explain this: studies with a negative effect size predominately assessed eating-disorder prevalence in athletes versus non-athletes, whereas the studies with positive effect sizes assessed exercise in eating disordered versus non-disordered women. The relationships between exercise and AN and BN could not be explored due to insufficient studies including data obtained from a clinically defined sample compared directly to a healthy group in each diagnostic case.

Substance use

Disturbed eating was associated with overall substance use level (r = .17), but the result was not robust and showed various effect sizes. This may be explained by the assessment measure utilised: Granner et al. ( 16 ), who used only the EDI behavioural sub-scales, found a much stronger association than researchers who used the entire scale (r =.43 versus mean weighted r =.14) suggesting that non-eating-disorder-specific behaviours may be contributing to the relationship more than the specific psychopathological aspects and that there are non-specific links between substance use and disturbed eating. Substance-specific analyses demonstrated that alcohol and smoking were loosely related to disturbed eating (r =.13 and r =.20 respectively). However, the illicit drug use analysis was homogeneous (r =.19), showing that such substance use is more common in women with disordered eating than in control women. While women with BN showed elevated use of all three types of substance use compared to control women, the relationships were not robust in the analysis and caution should be exercised when interpreting these results.

Socio-cultural pressures

As socio-cultural pressure is a multi-faceted concept, it was broken down into three areas: internalisation of the thin-ideal, the influence of media, and peer and family teasing/pressure. No association was found between internalisation and disturbed eating (r = .11), although cluster analysis revealed that two studies ( 17 r = - .06, 18 r = - .29) masked the medium association found in the remaining six studies (mean r = .37). These two negative effect sizes may be caused firstly by age – Share and Mintz ( 17 ) used a sample with a higher average age than those used in all the other studies – and secondly by assessment measure: Miotto et al. ( 18 ) were the only group to use the Marlow-Crown social desirability scale, which may exert a unique effect on the association. Media influence showed a small association with disturbed eating (r =.14) but with varying heterogeneity. However, the varied effect sizes disappeared when the Lin and Kulik ( 19 ) study was removed, which assessed dissatisfaction with body-parts rather than overall body dissatisfaction. Finally, a strong, robust association (r =.60) was found between disordered eating and teasing about weight and shape by family and friends, with varied effect sizes noted. The Irving study ( 20 ), one of two studies which found much smaller effect sizes (r = .16) compared women with high BULIT scale scores to those with low scores, potentially resulting in the control group having a higher degree of disturbed eating than the control groups of all the other studies which included only women from the general population.

Race

There was no association between race and the presence of disturbed eating behaviours or attitudes in this very large analysis (r = .02). However, to make the analysis more meaningful, the specific question of whether disturbed eating is associated more with Caucasian women than women from other ethnic groups was explored. A series of five analyses were conducted which found that disturbed eating was not associated more with being a female Caucasian than being a female from an African (African-American and African-British are being referred to as African), Asian (overall), South Asian (based on the British usage of “Asian”, referring to populations from countries such as India, Pakistan, Bangladesh), North Asian (based on the American usage, referring to populations coming from countries such as China, Japan, Taiwan), or Hispanic ethnicity (r = .05, r = -.02, r = -.09, r = .07, r = -.01 respectively). Based on the large sample in this review (N = 30,926), we conclude that race has no association with the presence of disturbed eating behaviours or attitudes.

Individual differences

As such a large number of individual difference factors have been linked to eating disorders in the literature, we divided our analyses into 12 sub-studies.

Defence style: Although an association between an immature defence style and disturbed eating was found within the reviewed studies (r = .30), significant heterogeneity was found among the effect sizes. More research is recommended to strengthen an understanding of this relationship.

Autonomy: Problems surrounding autonomy (subjective feelings of choice and freedom) were associated with disturbed eating, but there only appears to be an association between autonomy deficits and disordered eating in specific clinical cases. Effect sizes were mixed between studies and likely due to the specificity of eating disorder diagnosis. While BN showed a robust association with autonomy deficits (r = .42), there was not enough study data to permit an analysis for AN.

Shame and guilt: Our analysis found a moderate association between disturbed eating and feelings of shame and guilt (r = .29), with heterogeneous effect sizes. The cluster analysis showed that a much stronger effect size was seen in the study that included participants with specific clinical diagnoses ( 21 ), whereas a smaller effect size was found in the one study ( 22 ) that used the shame and guilt section from a generalised personality measure rather than the specifically developed shame and guilt measures used by the other five studies. Associations with clinical diagnoses were not possible due to a lack of studies.

Perfectionism: There was a moderate association between perfectionism and disturbed eating (r = .30), with mixed effect sizes. Composition of the sample in terms of clinical specificity appeared to be affecting the heterogeneity, but as the association between AN and perfectionism could not be conducted, this was not possible to confirm.

Anger and hostility: Disturbed eating was related to anger and hostility (r = .29), but individual study effect sizes varied. For example, there was a larger effect size in the Williams et al. ( 21 ) study, which used only clinically diagnosed women rather than a combination of clinical and sub-clinical participants as compared to other studies. Although both AN and BN were found to have a significant relationship with anger and hostility (r = .13, r = .36), the calculated effect sizes were again elevated by the Williams et al. ( 21 ) study. This suggests it cannot be specificity of diagnosis alone that is causing the heterogeneity in the effect sizes. Further exploration showed that Williams et al. ( 21 ) had utilised a measure of hostility (the Hostility and Direction of Hostility Questionnaire ( 23 )) whereas the other studies had used the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI ( 24 )) which measures anger. Our initial decision to combine these two concepts may have, therefore, been misguided. The analyses were re-run, resulting in anger showing a clear association with disturbed eating, but the association with hostility remaining heterogeneous indicating that it may be moderated by disorder specificity. However, as all the studies found positive relationships, varying only in magnitude, additional research is needed to describe more precisely the nature of this relationship.

Coping: Maladaptive coping style showed a small association with disturbed eating (r = .18), with varied effect sizes between individual studies; a similar association was found between BN and maladaptive coping (r = .17). As both analyses were not robust in terms of the fail safe N number and given the heterogeneous effect sizes, we suggest further research is conducted to help clarify the emerging association between maladaptive coping and disordered eating.

Temperament: There was a small association between temperament and disturbed eating (r = .17). However, this result was not robust and showed heterogeneous effect sizes possibly moderated by clinical diagnosis. There was a large association between AN and temperament, albeit with varied effect sizes between studies (r = .46), suggesting that a clinical diagnosis is not solely responsible for the heterogeneity found in the initial overall analysis. Additionally, there was no association between BN and temperament, although this was not robust. We therefore suggest more research would be useful to gain insights into the characteristics that may be responsible for the heterogeneity of effect sizes found in these analyses.

Anxiety: Overall, there was strong evidence for an association (r = .47) between disturbed eating and anxiety (the definition of which was not restricted to clinical diagnosis), although the effect sizes varied, again potentially due to whether studies had used clinically diagnosed samples. The largest effect sizes were found in studies that had used only clinically diagnosed patients, whereas those with smaller effect sizes had also used sub-clinical women. However, although both AN and BN were strongly linked with anxiety (r = .56, r = .57 respectively), effect sizes still varied significantly, suggesting clinical diagnosis may not be the sole moderator. We recommend additional research to explore the potential moderators affecting effect sizes between the studies to date.

Depression: This substantial analysis revealed a large and robust association between depression and disturbed eating (r = .39). However, although effect sizes ranged from r = .01 to r = .91, potential moderators could not be identified. The specific analyses with AN and BN were similar: large associations (r = .75, r = .67 respectively), but also substantial inexplicable heterogeneity. Therefore, it can be concluded that despite some variation in association strength between studies, speculatively caused by methodological and definition differences, research suggests that those with disordered eating show higher levels of depression than control women.

Personality traits: Sufficient data were only available to focus on the associations that extroversion/introversion and neuroticism have with disturbed eating. No association between extraversion (r = .04) or neuroticism (r = -.04) and disturbed eating was found. Although the effect sizes in the former were homogeneous, those in the latter showed mixed effect sizes, with the Davis, Claridge and Cerullo ( 25 ) study responsible for lowering the overall effect size to one of no association. Underlying reasons, however, could not be identified from Davis et al.'s paper.

Personality disorders: The presence of a personality disorder was more likely in women displaying disturbed eating than in control women (r = .26). However, effect sizes varied, with one out of the 27 differing in direction, and the analysis suggesting that personality disorders may only be related to the clinical forms of eating disorders. Indeed, co-morbidity does seem more likely when the specific clinical disorders of AN and BN are considered (r = .32, r = .34 respectively), although effect sizes still varied between studies. Three specific personality disorders were sufficiently investigated to allow separate meta-analyses: narcissism, obsessive-compulsiveness, and borderline personality. Although there was a degree of variation between the studies' findings, there is a tendency for personality disorders to be seen more in women with both general disturbed eating and the specific eating disorders. A clear, robust association showed that women with disturbed eating are more likely to suffer from borderline personality than control women (r = .30). Narcissism also showed an association with disturbed eating (r = .30), but the effect sizes displayed heterogeneity with two studies finding no notable association and appearing responsible for variation, as when removed a significant association remained. Finally, there appears to be a tendency for eating disordered women to show more obsessive-compulsivity than healthy women (r = .27), with this relationship being stronger in women with AN (r = .33) than in women with BN (r = .28). Effect sizes varied, however, with moderators not readily identifiable.

Self-esteem: Analyses revealed no association between low self-esteem and disturbed eating (r = .03) or BN (r = -.01). However, there was a large association between low self-esteem and AN (r = .47), although the effect sizes were mixed, with one study ( 21 ) showing a large negative association and all the others large positive associations. Reasons for Williams's study standing out could not be identified. Overall, data suggested women with AN, but not with general disturbances or BN, are more likely to have lowered self-esteem compared to healthy women. We suggest more research is conducted in this area to help explain the mixed findings.

Discussion

The purpose of this review was to consolidate, using meta-analyses, the vast body of retrospective eating disorder associate-factor literature published between 1987 and 2003 to identify which of the many factors linked with eating disorders show a true association with general problematic eating and the clinical forms of AN and BN.

In total, 74 meta-analyses (47 main analyses and 27 follow-ups) were conducted on 12 main areas, comprising 232 published studies involving 87,878 participants. Only 14 of all the analyses (19%) resulted in clear associations (see Table 2 for a descriptive summary of the results), highlighting the range of study and participant features that vary across the research. A further 38 analyses (51%) found agreement on the direction of effects, differing only on the magnitude of the association.

In keeping with a continuity perspective (e.g., 26 , 27 ), it was expected that the analyses investigating the specific clinical eating disorders would show larger associations with each factor than general disturbed eating. This was found to be true in many cases: in seven analyses at least one of the clinical diagnoses showed higher associations and in six analyses both diagnoses had stronger associations than did general disturbed eating. These findings support those of Stice ( 1 ), who reported that studies predicting general disordered eating had smaller effect sizes than studies focused on specific eating disorders. Using the results in combination, it can be speculated that these levels would be higher if analyses comparing clinical AN and BN with controls had been possible for each analysis.

As expected from the literature, the results also showed that although the diagnoses of AN and BN do share common relationships with factors, they also demonstrated disorder-specific relationships with associates. For example, both AN and BN had strong links with cognitions, anxiety and depression, but BN additionally showed a strong relationship with poor caregiver relationships, and AN with temperament and low self-esteem. Clinically this is important as it highlights both the overlapping and unique aspects of the disorders allowing a profile of each disorder to be constructed which in turn can lead to disorder specific strategies to be developed in terms of both treatment and prevention. Additionally, it can inform further research on the interplay of the specific factors to determine the weight each individual factor contributes to the specific diagnosis when in combination with all the other factors. Again this would be valuable to both treatment and prevention efforts.

Figure 2
Table 2: Summary of the overall strength of the associations between each factor and the three main outcomes

The results from this series of meta-analytic studies justify the decision to extend the meta-analysis of Stice ( 1 ) demonstrating that the vast amount of research on eating disorder associates can make a very useful contribution to the body of knowledge on this topic. The available literature resulted in the two studies covering, to a degree, different factors; some of the factors investigated in this series of studies simply were not covered by the Stice analyses, presumably due to the lack of prospective research available. Therefore although this review consolidates some of the findings from the Stice review ( 1 ), reviewing the retrospective research also adds information to the general discussion and highlights areas where further longitudinal work would be very useful.

This series of meta-analytic studies resulted in a number of important findings. The first is that race has no association with disordered eating, that is being Caucasian carries no more risk of problematic eating than being of any other ethnic origin. As this analysis included data from over 31,000 women, our conclusion adds substantial weight to the long-standing debate in this area, and suggests that in detecting eating disorders, clinicians should not assume Caucasians are more at risk. It must be noted, however, that the analyses presented were intentionally concerned only with race: that is, colour of skin, with no account being made of culture or country of origin, which could be addressed in future work. A further two striking findings were the substantial relationships between all forms of eating disorder with anxiety and depression. One can give extra weight and confidence for these findings as they included exceptionally large sample sizes. However, the results still do not answer the question of whether the eating disorders are variants of these two co-morbid disorders, or are separate entities that share commonalties.

A surprising finding was the mixed results of the self-esteem analyses, which showed a wholly inconclusive relationship with general disordered eating, no relationship with BN, but a strong varied association with AN. An obvious link between low self-esteem and disturbed eating in general has been presumed based on the assumption that the individual must feel their body is inadequate in order to desire to change it so desperately ( 28 ), and feeling inadequate goes hand-in-hand with low self-esteem. However, based on our analyses, this relationship may only apply in the case of AN. It is likely the surprising result is due to methodological differences between the studies investigating this concept – for example, whether self-esteem was treated as a uni- or multi-dimensional concept. It is plausible that self-esteem is related to general eating disturbance and BN, but there needs to be more methodological rigour to detect the associations with these outcomes, which may be weaker than that found with AN.

The socio-cultural pressure analyses also provided interesting results. Despite the stressed importance of media influence and internalisation of the thin-ideal in the literature and in popular media, they surprisingly showed little association with disordered eating. However, this could be due to desensitisation caused by overexposure to the thin-ideal and/or a clearer understanding that many magazine pictures are altered. Additionally, when assessment measure was taken into account, it appeared that assessing dissatisfaction with body-parts ( 19 ) rather than overall body dissatisfaction lowered the overall effect. The media tend to promote an overall desirable body shape, rather than focusing on separate parts of the body, and so it appears that studies focusing on the latter may be missing the important aspects of this concept.

Our analyses demonstrated a strong association between disordered eating and weight- and shape-related teasing. This is an interesting finding as it suggests that weight/shape teasing, which in varying levels may feature commonly in the everyday lives of some adolescent girls, may have lasting implications and lead to a general level of disturbed eating.

The inductive moderator searches conducted when effect sizes showed heterogeneity revealed some interesting insights. In the family environment analysis it was suggested that as one gets older, family dysfunction no longer places one at an increased risk of eating disturbance. This may be due to no longer living with the family unit, or that memory of poor family environment diminishes with age. Similarly, in the sexuality analysis it was shown that age and culture may explain the variance in effect sizes. The data suggested that Australian lesbian women may perceive less weight and shape pressures in comparison to American lesbian women, and additionally, lesbian women overall may become less concerned with weight and shape as they get older to a greater extent than do heterosexual women overall.

The medium-strength relationship between disturbed eating and self-harm behaviours was found to be affected by the assessment measures used. It appears that when a measure specific for self harm is used the resultant effect is smaller, suggesting that non-specific measures may be tapping into other phenomena associated with disturbed eating, and therefore to gain the true effect, a measure specifically designed for the purpose should be used. In the substance use analysis, associations were detected between disturbed eating and smoking and illegal drug use, but not between disturbed eating and alcohol use. This could be due to a perception that alcohol is a weight-gaining substance, whereas cigarettes and some illegal drugs are perceived to have a weight-reducing effect. Finally, in the exercise analysis it was found that studies reporting a negative effect size predominately assessed eating-disorder prevalence in athletes versus non-athletes, whereas the studies with positive effect sizes assessed exercise in eating disordered versus non-disordered women. Although not wholly clear-cut due to some reminaing heterogentity in the analysis, this suggests that women with disordered eating appear to exercise more than other women, but athletes are no more likely to have an eating disorder than non-athlete controls.

Meta-analysis has its limitations. We are sensitive that the findings reported in this paper are dependent on the methods and decisions we applied (i.e., our decisions to use Fishers z-transformation, a random-effects model, to consider only published studies). Had alternative methodological decisions been made (using a fixed-effects model for example), it is possible our results may have differed. We would like to emphasize that the methods and criteria applied are consistent with the standard practices of meta-analysis, and that in most cases the decisions provided the most conservative estimates of effects, and so if our results are skewed in any way it is likely that we have made Type II errors rather than Type I.

A second limitation of the analyses is connected with the multi-dimensional nature of many of the measures used in the aggregated studies: for example, the restraint scale ( 29 ) measures dieting behaviour, binge eating and weight fluctuation. The decision in this analysis to average scores across all sub-scales to produce one effect size for each study means that how the specific dimensions vary in their contribution to the effects found was not considered. Stice ( 1 ) suggests that for a clearer interpretation of study findings, measures that assess concepts in a unitary fashion are needed rather than multidimensional measures. Additionally, there are potential problems with assuming a consistent definition for measures such as body dissatisfaction. Studies use and assess this outcome in different ways, and where one study may assess BD as a behavioural measure, another may assess it as an attitude. While this is a limitation of this meta-analysis, it is also a limitation of the research collated, as the majority of the studies did not clearly define such constructs. Further consolidation of how concepts are defined and measured in the eating disorder field is needed to provide analytic clarity to study findings.

A third limitation of the study is that data was abstracted from all relevant studies with no methodological assessment of the studies in which the data came from. This approach was taken as it was felt important to get the broadest summary of the non-longitudinal data as possible. The main study characteristics were coded using the developed coding scheme in the hope that if methodological heterogeneity existed between the studies it would have been detected by the inductive moderator searchers performed for each analysis. However, in a number of the analyses heterogeneity could not be explained by the characteristics coded for. To strengthen our observations of associations, in retrospect it would have been preferable to assess the included studies for methodological quality, especially in terms of control of confounders; the likely cause of the varied effect sizes.

We must mention that the main reason studies were excluded from these analyses was due to a lack of appropriate statistics: 33% of the excluded studies (150 studies) were left out for this reason. There was a surprising lack of inferential statistics in many of the viewed studies, and even commonplace descriptive statistics, such as means and standard deviations, were often not reported; many studies gave only percentages, which were unable to be used for the particular effect size approach used in our meta-analysis. We found the standard of reporting in the literature lacking and hope our experience of examining the whole research corpus serves as a reminder to researchers in this field that quantitative data can be used beyond the particular study, and so should always be reported as fully as possible.

As a final point, the results of this meta-analysis are mainly based on retrospective methods of data collection, which are open to respondent bias, and also highlight associations or relationships between eating disturbance and various factors, and do not give the ability to derive interpretations regarding cause and effect. Many of the factors investigated could just as plausibly be consequences of the disorder, rather than antecedents, and so it cannot be concluded that these relationships are evidence that the factor precedes the onset of the disorder. The very nature of this review in taking retrospective, correlational studies for analysis automatically limits the review findings to the same correlational, retrospective level. To confirm, for example, that people with BN experience more stress than healthy women rather than just report/perceive more, or to allow causality to be implied, further prospective, longitudinal work must be conducted. We hope our analysis will be valuable for guiding such efforts.

Conclusions

Based on the findings from this series of meta-analytic studies it is possible to sketch a profile of women with disturbed eating, AN and BN. Women with disordered eating are: more likely than controls to have experienced abuse, especially sexual abuse; have a dysfunctional attachment, and a tendency to come from a more dysfunctional family background; to have higher levels of distorted/irrational cognitions; to have experienced weight- or shape-related teasing; to more likely utilise maladaptive coping strategies and immature defences, to smoke and take illegal drugs, but be no more likely to use alcohol; are perfectionists who show higher levels of anger and hostility, shame and guilt, neuroticism, and have anxious, depressive, borderline, and obsessive-compulsive traits. However, women with disordered eating are not more likely to: be of any given sexuality; self-harm; have higher levels of stress; exercise; or be more, or less, extroverted in their behaviour; be more likely to internalise norms, or differ in temperament. In addition to this overall profile, women with AN and BN are more likely than women with general disordered eating to have dysfunctional attachments, to be depressed and anxious, and to have higher levels of obsessive compulsive disorders; they also show higher levels of irrational cognitions, anger and hostility. Further, women with BN are more likely than women with disturbed eating and AN to come from families with dysfunctional backgrounds, be stressed and show reduced autonomy, whereas women with AN are more likely than women with disturbed eating and BN to have high levels of general personality disorders, and low self-esteem.

With regard to direction for future longitudinal and experimental research, our suggestions fall into three categories. First, the results of the race, and temperament analyses suggest that no more research, either longitudinal or correlational, is needed, as these are simply not associated with eating pathology. Second, certain factors should receive more cross-sectional research, or further investigation of the association moderators, before money and time are invested on large-scale prospective studies. Specifically, self-harm, irrational beliefs, anger and hostility, coping, and self-esteem could potentially benefit from further investigation of the moderators involved in the associations, or further work on the definition/measurement of the concepts, whilst defence style, personality traits, and borderline personality disorder would all benefit from further correlational work due to the small number of studies currently available. Third, there are factors that are deemed important and interesting enough to warrant the investment of time and money in prospective and experimental studies. There are promising associations between abuse and family environment and the different forms of eating pathology. Despite these areas being assumed to have a role in eating pathology development, Stice ( 1 ) found few longitudinal studies on these areas and as such it would be highly beneficial to investigate these two areas in an empirical manner. Moreover, autonomy, negative cognitions, shame and guilt, attachment with primary caregiver, exercise and obsession-compulsion were all strongly associated with eating disturbance in this review but were not assessed by Stice ( 1 ), so offer further scope for longitudinal investigation. Finally, there were some factors in this review, which although associated with eating pathology and previously assessed by Stice ( 1 ), would still merit further prospective investigation due to a lack of studies on specific associations with AN or BN. For example, the various concepts of socio-cultural pressure and substance use have been suggested to be related in different ways to each form of disorder, and determining whether these speculations are correct would enrich the profiles of the two disorders.

{image:3}

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by a PhD fellowship from the ESRC. The authors would like to thank Dr. Christopher Eccleston and Dr. Sarah Milne for their advice on meta-analysis procedures.

Correspondence to

Dr. Emma Vince, Department of Primary Care and General Practice, University of Birmingham, Edgbaston Birmingham, B15 2TT, United Kingdom. E-mail: E.P.Vince@bham.ac.uk Fax: +44 (0)121 4146571

References followed by * are those included in the meta-analyses.

References

1. Stice E. Risk and maintenance factors for eating pathology: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull 2002; 128: 825-848.
2. Garner DM., Olmsted M, Polivy J. Development and validation of a multidimensional eating disorders inventory for anorexia nervosa and bulimia. Int J Eat Disord 1983; 2: 15-34.
3. Garner DM, Garfinkel P.E. The eating attitudes test: An index of the symptoms of anorexia nervosa. Psychol Med 1979; 9: 273-279.
4. Schwarzer R. Meta-Analysis [Computer software and manual]. 1989; Berlin, Germany: Freie Universität Berlin.
5. Hunter JE, Schmidt, FL, Jackson GB. Meta-analysis: Cumulating Research Findings across Studies. Studying Organizations Series; Innovations in Methodology, 1982; 4; London; Sage Publications, in cooperation with Division 14 of the APA.
6. Hedges LV, Olkin I. Statistical Methods for Meta-analysis. 1985; New York: Academic Press.
7. Silver NC, Dunlap WP. Averaging correlation coefficients: Should Fisher's Z transformation be used? J Appl Psychol, 1987; 72: 146-148.
8. Cramer D, Howitt D. The Sage Dictionary of Statistics. 2004; London: Sage
9. Rosenthal R. Judgement Studies: Design, Analysis and Meta-analysis. 1987; Cambridge; Cambridge University Press.
10. Cohen JD. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd Ed). 1988; New York: Academic Press.
11. Rosenthal R. Meta Analytic Procedures for Social Research. 1984; Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
12. Lehoux P, Steiger H, Jabalpurwala S. State/trait distinctions in bulimic syndromes. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 27: 36-42.*
13. Waller G, Slade P, Calam R. Who knows best? Family interactions and eating disorders. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 156: 546-550.*
14. Scalf-McIver L, Thompson J. Family correlates of bulimic characteristics in college females. J Clin Psychol 1989; 45: 467-472.*
15. Cooper M, Turner H. Underlying assumptions and core beliefs in anorexia nervosa and dieting. Br J Clin Psychol 2000; 39: 215-218*.
16. Granner M, Black D, Abood D. Levels of cigarette and alcohol use related to eating disorder attitudes. Am J Health Behav 2002; 26: 43-55.*
17. Share T, Mintz L. Differences between Lesbians and heterosexual women in disordered eating and related attitudes. J Homosex 2002; 42: 89-106.*
18. Miotto P, DeCoppi M, Frezza M, Rossi M, Preti A. Social desirability and eating disorder: a community study of an Italian school-ages sample. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2002; 105: 372-377.*
19. Lin L, Kulik J. Social comparison and womens body satisfaction. Basic Appl Soc Psychol 2002; 24: 115-123.*
20. Irving LM. Mirror images: Effects of the standard of beauty on self, and body esteem. J Soc Clin Psychol 1990; 9: 230-242.*
21. Williams G, Power K, Millar H, Freeman C, Yellowlees A, Dowds T, Walker M, Campsie L, MacPherson F, Jackson M. Comparison of eating disorders and other dietary/ weight groups on measures of perceived control, assertiveness, self-esteem and self-directed hostility. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 14: 27-32.*
22. Augestad L, Saether B, Gotestam K. The relationship between eating disorders and personality in physically active women. Scand J Med Sci Sports 1999; 9: 304-312.*
23. Caine TM, Foulds G.A, Hope K. Manual of the Hostility and Direction of Hostility Questionnaire. 1967; London: University of London Press
24. Speilberger CD. State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory: Professional Manual. 1966; Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
25. Davis C, Claridge G, Cerullo D. Personality factors and weight preoccupation: a continuum approach to the association between eating disorders and personality disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1997; 31: 467-480.*
26. Gleaves D, Lowe M, Snow A, Green B, Murphy-Eberenz K. Continuity and discontinuity models of bulimia nervosa: A taxometric investigation. J Abnorm Psychol 2000; 109: 56-68.
27. Ruderman A, Besbeas M. Psychological characteristics of dieters and bulimics. J Abnorm Psychol 1992; 101: 383-390.
28. Palmer R. Helping People with Eating Disorders: A Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment. 2000; Chichester: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.
29. Heatherton TF, Herman CP, Polvy J, King GA, McGree ST. The (mis) measurement of restraint: An analysis of conceptual and psychometric issues. J Abnorm Psychol 1988; 97: 19-28
30. Abood D, Chandler S. Race and the role of weight, weight change and body dissatisfaction in eating disorders. Am J Health Behav 1997; 21: 21-25.*
31. Abrahms KK, La Rue, Gray JJ. Disordered eating attitudes and behaviours, psychological adjustment and ethinic identity: Comparison of black and white females. Int J Eat Disord 1993; 14: 49-57.*
32. Abramson EE, Lucido GM. Childhood sexual experiences and bulimia nervosa, Addict Behav 1991; 16: 529-32.*
33. Ackard D, Brehm B, Steffen J. Exercise and eating disorders in college-aged women; Profiling excessive exercisers. Eat Disord 2002; 10: 31-47.*
34. Ackard D, Croll J, Cooke A. Dieting frequency among college females: Association with disordered eating, body image and related psychological problems. J Psychosom Res 2002; 52: 129-136.*
35. Ahmad S, Waller G, Verduyn C. Eating attitudes among Asian schoolgirls: the role of perceived parental control. Int J Eat Disord 1994; 15: 91-97.*
36. Akan G. Grilo C. Socio-cultural influences on eating attitudes and behaviours, body image, and psychological functioning; A comparison of African-American, Asian-American, and Caucasian college women. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 18: 181-187.*
37. Anderluh M, Tchanturia K, Rabe-Hesketh S, Treasure J. Childhood obsessive-compulsive personality traits in adult women with eating disorder: Defining a broader eating disorder phenotype. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 247.*
38. Andrews B, Valentine E, Valentine J. Depression and eating disorders following abuse in childhood in two generations of women. Br J Psychol 1995; 34: 37-52.*
39. Arriaza C, Mann T. Ethnic differences in eating disorder symptoms among college students: the confounding role of body mass index. J ACH 2001; 49: 309-315.*
40. Ashby J, Kottman T, Schoen E. Perfectionism and eating disorder reconsidered. J Ment Health Couns 1998; 20: 261-271.*
41. Atlas J, Smith G, Hohlstein L, McCarthy D, Kroll L. Similarities and differences between Caucasian and African American college women on eating and dieting expectancies, bulimic symptoms, dietary restraint and disinhibition. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 32: 326-334.*
42. Ball K, Lee C, Brown W. Psychological stress and disordered eating: An exploratory study with young Australian women. Women Health 1999; 29: 1-15.*
43. Bastiani A, Rao R, Weltzin T, Kaye W. Perfectionism in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 17: 147-152.*
44. Bekker M, Boselie K. Gender and stress: Is gender role stress? A re-examination of the relationship between feminine gender role stress and eating disorders. Stress Health 2002; 18: 141-149.*
45. Bennett D, Cooper C. Psychological discriminators of subjects at different stages of the eating disturbed spectrum. Pers Individ Dif 2001; 30: 917-929.*
46. Beren S, Hayden H, Wilfley D, Grilo C. The influence of sexual orientation on body dissatisfaction in adult men and women. Int J Eat Disord 1996; 20: 135-141.*
47. Blaase H, Elklit A. Psychological characteristics of women with eating disorder: permanent or transient features? Scand J Psychol 2001; 42: 467-478.*
48. Blouin A, Zuro C, Blouin J. Family environment in Bulimia nervosa: The role of depression. Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 649-658.*
49. Breaux C, Moreno J. Comparing anorectics and bulimics on measures of depression, anxiety and anger. Eat Disord 1994; 2: 158-167.*
50. Brookings J, Wilson J. Personality and family environment predictors of self-reported eating attitudes and behaviours. J Pers Assess 1994; 63: 313-326.*
51. Brooks-Gunn J, Burrow C, Warren M. Attitudes toward eating and body weight in different groups of female adolescent athletes. Int J Eat Disord 1988; 7: 749-757.*
52. Bulik C, Beidel D, Duchmann E, Weltzin T, Kaye, W. An analysis of social anxiety in anorectic, bulimic, social phobic and control women. J Psychopathol Behav Assess 1991; 13: 199-211.*
53. Button E, Loan P, Davies J, Sonuga-Barke E. Self-esteem, eating problems and psychological well-being in a cohort of schoolgirls aged 15-16: A questionnaire and interview study. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 39-47.*
54. Button E, Reveley C, Palmer R. An ethnic comparison of eating attitudes and associated psychological problems in young British Women. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 23: 317-323.*
55. Bybee J, Zigler E. Guilt, guilt-evoking events, depression, and eating disorders. Curr Psychol 1996; 15: 113.*
56. Cachelin F, Striegel-Moore R, Paget W. Comparison of women with various levels of dietary restraint on body image, personality, and family environment. Eat Disord 1997; 5: 205-215.*
57. Cachelin F, Weiss J, Garbanati J. Dieting and its relationship to smoking, acculturation, and family environment in Asian and Hispanic Adolescents. Eat Disord 2003; 11: 51-61.*
58. Calam R, Slade P. Eating problems and sexual experiences: Some relationships. Br Rev Bul Anorexia Nervosa 1987; 2: 37-43.*
59. Calam R, Slade P. Sexual experiences and eating problems in female undergraduates. Int J Eat Disord 1989; 8: 391-397. *
60. Caldwell M, Brownell K, Wilfley D. Relationship of weight, body dissatisfaction and self-esteem in African American and White female dieters. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 22: 127-130.*
61. Caradas A, Lambert E, Charlton K. An ethnic comparison of eating attitudes and associated body image concerns in adolescent South African schoolgirls. J Humn Nutr Diet 2001; 14: 111-120.*
62. Carroll J, Touyz S, Beumont P. Specific co-morbidity between bulimia nervosa and personality disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1996; 19: 159-170.*
63. Casper RC. Lyubomirsky S. Individual psychopathology relative to reports of unwanted sexual experiences as predictor of a bulimic eating pattern. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 229-236.*
64. Chandler S, Abood D, Lee D, Cleveland M, Daly J. Pathogenic eating attitudes and behaviours and body dissatisfaction differences among Black and white college students. Eat Disord 1994; 2: 319-328.*
65. Claes L, Vandereycken W, Vertommen H. Impulsive and compulsive traits in eating disordered patients compared with controls. Pers Individ Dif 2002; 32: 707-714.*
66. Claridge G, Davis C, Bellhouse M, Kaptein S. Borderline personality, nightmares and adverse life events in the risk for eating disorders. Pers Individ Dif 1998; 25: 339-351.*
67. Clark D, Feldman J, Channon S. Dysfunctional thinking in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Cog Ther Res 1989; 13: 377-387.*
68. Cockell S, Hewitt P, Seal B, Sherry S, Goldner E, Flett G, Remick, R. Trait and self-presentational dimensions of perfectionism among women with anorexia nervosa. Cog Ther Res 2002; 26: 745-758.*
69. Cole-Detke H, Kobak R. Attachment processes in eating disorder and depression. J Consult Clin Psychol 1996; 64: 282-290.*
70. Cooper CL. Contents, origins and consequences of dysfunctional beliefs in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. J Cog Psychotherapy 1998; 12: 213-230.*
71. Cooper M, Galbraith M, Drinkwater J. Assumptions and beliefs in adolescents with anorexia nervosa and their mothers. Eat Disord 2001; 9: 217-223.*
72. Cooper M, Hunt J. Core beliefs and underlying assumptions in Bulimia nervosa and depression. Beh Res Ther 1998; 36: 895-898.*
73. Cooper M, Todd G. Selective processing of three types of stimuli in eating disorders. Br J Psychol 1997; 36: 279-281.*
74. Crawford S, Eklund R. Social physique anxiety, reasons for exercise, and attitudes toward exercise settings. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1994; 16: 70-82.*
75. Crowther J, Kichler J, Sherwood N, Kuhnert M. The role of familial factors in bulimia nervosa. Eat Disord 2002; 10: 141-151.*
76. Cusumano D, Thompson J. Body image and body shape ideal in magazines: exposure, awareness and internalisation. Sex Roles 1997; 37: 701-721.*
77. Dacey C, Nelson W, Aikman K. Prevalencey rate and personality comparisons of bulimic and normal adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 1990; 20: 243-251.*
78. Davis C. Body Image, dieting behaviours, and personality factors: a study of high-performance female athletes. Int J Sport Psychol 1992; 23: 179-192.*
79. Davis C. Body image and weight preoccupation: a comparison between exercising and non-exercising women. Appetite 1990; 15: 13-21.*
80. Davis C, Claridge G, Cerullo D. Reflections on narcissism: conflicts about body-image perceptions in women. Pers Individ Dif 1997; 22: 309-316.*
81. Davis C, Cowles M. A comparison of weight and diet concerns and personality factors among female athletes and non-athletes. J Psychosom Res 1989; 33: 527-536.*
82. Davis C, Fox J, Cowles M, Hastings P, Schwass K. The functional role of exercise in the development of weight and diet concerns in women. J Psychosom Res 1990; 34: 563-574.*
83. Davis C, Kennedy S, Ravelski E, Dionne M. The role of physical activity in the development and maintenance of eating disorders. Psychol Med 1994; 24: 957-967.*
84. Deep A, Lilenfeld L, Plotnicov K, Pollice C, Kaye W. Sexual abuse in eating disorder sub-types and control women: the role of co-morbid substance dependence in bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1999; 25: 1-10.*
85. Denisoff E, Endler N. Life experiences, coping and weight preoccupation in young adult women. Can J Behav Sci 2000; 32: 97-103.*
86. Diaz-Marsa M. A study of temperament and personality in anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa. J Personal Disord 2000; 14: 352-359.*
87. Diehl N, Johnson C, Rogers R, Petrie T. Social physique anxiety and disordered eating: Whats the connection. Addict Behav 1998; 23: 1-6.*
88. Dolan B, Lacey J, Evans C. Eating behaviour and attitudes to weight and shape in British women from three ethnic groups. Br J Psychiatry 1990; 157: 523-528.*
89. Fahy T. Obsessive-compulsive symptoms in eating disorders. Beh Res Ther 1991; 29: 113-116.*
90. Fassino S, Abbate-Daga G, Amianto F, Leombruni P, Boggio S, Rovera G. Temperament and character profile of eating disorders: A controlled study with the temperament and character inventory. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 32: 412-425.*
91. Fassino S, Daga G, Piero A, Leombruni P, Rovera G. Anger and personality in eating disorders. J Psychosom Res 2001; 51: 757-764.*
92. Fassino S, Svrakic D, Abbate-Daga G, Leombruni P, Amianto F, Stanic S, Rovera G. Anorectic family dynamics: Temperament and character data. Compr Psychiatry 2002; 43: 114-120.*
93. Felkner K, Stivers C. The relationship of gender and family environment to eating disorder risk in adolescents. Adolescence 1994; 29: 821-834.*
94. Fischer M. Eating attitudes, health-risk behaviours and self-esteem and anxiety among adolescent females. J Adolesc Health 1991; 12: 377-384.*
95. Formea G, Burns G. Relation between the syndromes of bulimia nervosa and obsessive compulsive disorders. J Psychopathol Behav Assess1995; 17: 167-176.*
96. Frank, E. Shame and guilt in eating disorders. Am J Orthopsychiatry 1991; 61: 303-306.*
97. Frank S, Jackson S. Family experiences as moderators of the relationship between eating symptoms and personality disturbance. J Youth Adolesc 1996; 25: 55-72.*
98. Franko D, Herrera I. Body image differences in Gutemalan-American and white college women. Eat Disord 1997; 5: 119-127.*
99. Franko D, Omori M. Sub-clinical eating disorders in adolescent women: A test of the continuity hypothesis and it's psychological correlates. J Adolesc 1999; 22: 389-396.*
100. Frederick C, Grow V. A mediational model of autonomy, self-esteem, and eating disordered attitudes and behaviours. Psychol Women Q 1996; 20: 217-228.*
101. Frederick C, Morrison C. A mediational model of social physique anxiety and eating disordered behaviours. Percept Mot Skills 1998; 86: 139-145.*
102. Fryer S, Waller G, Kroese B. Stress, coping and disturbed eating attitudes in teenage girls. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 22: 427-436.*
103. Furnham A. Role of conflict with parents in disordered eating among British Asian females. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999; 34: 498-505.*
104. Ghadirian A, Scott B. Coping in dieting and eating disorders: a population based study. J Nerv Ment Dis 2000; 188: 273-279.*
105. Gilbert N, Meyer C. Social anxiety and social comparison: differential links with restrictive and bulimic attitudes among non-clinical women. Eat Behav 2003; 4: 257-264.*
106. Gleaves D, Williamson D, Fuller R. Bulimia nervosa symptomatology and body image disturbance associated with distance running and weight loss. Br J Sport Med 1992; 26: 157-160.*
107. Gluck M, Geliebter A. Racial/ethnic differences in body image and eating behaviours. Eat Behav,2002; 3: 143-151.*
108. Goebel M, Spalthoff G, Schulze C, Florin I. Dysfunctional cognitions, attributional style and depression in bulimia. J Psychosom Res 1989; 33: 747-752.*
109. Granner M, Abood D, Black D. Racial differences in eating disorder attitudes, cigarette and alcohol use. Am J Health Behav 2001; 25: 83-99.*
110. Griffiths R, Beumont P, Russell J, Schotte D, Thornton C, Touyz S, Varano P. Socio-cultural attitudes towards appearance in dieting disordered and non-dieting disordered subjects. Euro Eat Disord Rev 1999; 7: 193-203.*
111. Griffiths R, Mallia-Blanco R, Boesenberg E, Ellis C, Fischer K, Taylor M, Wyndham J. Restrained eating and socio-cultural attitudes to appearance and general dissatisfaction. Euro Eat Disord Rev 2000; 8: 394-402.*
112. Haase A, Prapavessis H. Social physique anxiety and eating attitudes in female athletic and non-athletic groups. J Sci Med Sport 2001; 4: 396-405.*
113. Harned M. Harassed bodies: an examination of the relationships among women's experiences of sexual harassment, body image, and eating disturbances. Psychol Women Q 2000; 24: 336-348.*
114. Haudek C, Rorty M, Henker B.The role of ethnicity and parental bonding in the eating and weight concerns of Asian-American and Caucasian college women. Int J Eat Disord 1999; 25: 425-433. *
115. Hausenblas H, Mack D. Social physique anxiety and eating disorder correlates among female athletic and non-athletic populations. J Sport Behav 1999; 22: 502-513.*
116. Hawkins W, McDermott R, Seeley J, Hawkins M. Depression and maladaptive eating practices in college students. Women Health 1992; 18: 55-67.*
117. Hewitt P, Flett G, Ediger E. Perfectionism traits and perfectionistic self-presentation in eating disorder attitudes, characteristics and symptoms. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 18: 317-326.*
118. Hinrichsen H, Wright F, Waller G, Meyer C. Social anxiety and coping strategies in the eating disorders. Eat Behav 2003; 4: 117-126.*
119. Imm P, Pruitt J. Body shape satisfaction in female exercisers and non-exercisers. Women Health 1991; 17: 87-95.*
120. James K, Phelps L, Bross A. Body dissatisfaction, drive for thinness and self-esteem in African-American college females. Psychol Sch 2001; 38: 491-496.*
121. Kanakis D, Thelen M. Parental variables associated with bulimia nervosa. Addict Behav 1995; 20: 491-500.*
122. Kaye WH, Weltzin TE, Hsu LG. Patients with anorexia nervosa have elevated scores on the Yale-Brown Obsessive-Compulsive scale. Int J Eat Disord 1992; 12: 57-62.*
123. Kenny M, Hart K. Relationships between parental attachment and eating disorders in an inpatient and a college sample. J Couns Psychol 1992; 39: 521-526.*
124. Kent A, Waller G, Dagnan D. Greater of emotional than physical or sexual abuse in predicting disordered eating attitudes: The role of mediating variables. Int J Eat Disord 1999; 25: 159-167.*
125. Kent J, Clopton J. Bulimic womens perceptions of their family relationships. J Clin Psychol 1992; 48: 281-292.*
126. Kichler J, Crowther J. The effects of maternal modelling and negative familial communication on women's eating attitudes and body image. Behav Ther 2001; 32: 443-457.*
127. Kleifield E, Sunday S, Hurt S, Halmi K. The Tri-demensional personality questionnaire: An exploration of personality traits in eating disorders. J Psychiatr Res 1994; 28: 413-423.*
128. Klump K, Bulik C, Pollice C, Halmi K, Fichter M, et al. Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa. J Nerv Ment Dis 2000; 188: 559-567.*
129. Koff E, Sangani P. Effects of coping style and negative body image on eating disturbance. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 22: 51-56.*
130. Krause E, Robins C, Lynch T. A mediational model relating sociotropy, ambivalence over emotional expression, and disordered eating. Psychol Women Q 2000; 24: 328-335.*
131. Kulbartz-Klatt Y, Pook M, Florin I. Bulimia nervosa: Mood changes so have an impact on body width estimation. Br J Psychol 1999; 38: 279-287.*
132. Lakkis J, Ricciardelli L, Williams R. Role of sexual orientation and gender-related traits in disordered eating. Sex Roles 1999; 41: 1-16.*
133. Laliberte M, Boland F, Leichner P. Family climates; family factors specific to disturbed eating and bulimia nervosa. J Clin Psychol 1999; 55: 1021-1041.*
134. Latzer Y, Hochdorf Z, Bachar E, Canetti L. Attachment style and family functioning as discriminating factors in eating disorders. Comtemp Fam Ther 2002; 24: 581-599.*
135. Leal L, Weise S, Dodd D. The relationship between gender, symptoms of bulimia, and tolerance for stress. Addict Behav 1995; 20: 105-109.*
136. Le Grange D, Stone A, Brownell K. Eating disturbances in White and minority female dieters. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 24: 395-403.*
137. Le Grange D, Telch C, Tibbs J. Eating attitudes and behaviours in 1,435 South African Caucasian and non-Caucasian college students. Am J Psychiat 1998; 155: 250-254.*
138. Leon G, Fulkerson J, Perry C, Dube A. Family Influences, school behaviours and risk for the later development of an eating disorder. J Youth Adolesc 1994; 23: 499-515.*
139. Lester R, Petrie T. Physical, psychological and societal correlates of bulimic symptomatology among African American College women. J Couns Psychol 1998; 45: 315-321.*
140. Leung N, Thomas G, Waller G. The relationship between parental bonding and core beliefs in anorexic and bulimic women. Br J Psychol 2000; 39: 205-213.*
141. Leung N, Waller G, Thomas G. Core beliefs in anorexic and bulimic women. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999; 187: 736-741.*
142. Lindeman M, Stark K. Emotional eating and eating disorder psychopathologyy. Eat Disord 2001; 9: 251-259.*
143. Lohr J, Parkinson D. Irrational beliefs and bulimia symptoms. J Rat Emot Cog Behav Ther 1989; 7: 253-262.*
144. Long C, Smith J, Midgley M, Cassidy T. Over-exercising in anorexic and normal samples; behaviour and attitudes. J Ment Health 1993; 2: 321-327.*
145. Lundholm J, Waters J. Dysfunctional family systems: relationship to disordered eating behaviours among university women. J Subst Abuse 1991; 3: 97-106.*
146. Mayhew R, Edelmann R.J. Self-esteem, irrational beliefs and coping strategies in relation to eating problems in a non-clinical population. Pers Individ Dif 1989; 10: 581-584. *
147. Mazzeo S, Espelage D. Association between childhood physical and emotional abuse and disordered eating behaviours in female undergraduates: An investigation of the mediating role of alexithymia and depression. J Couns Psychol 2002; 49: 86-100.*
148. McGrane D, Carr A. Young women at risk for eating disorders: perceived family dysfunction and parental psychological problems. Comtemp Fam Ther 2002; 24: 385-395.*
149. McLaren L, Gauvin L, Steiger H. A 2-factor model of disordered eating. Eat Behav 2001; 2: 51-65.*
150. McLean J, Barr S. Cognitive dietary restraint is associated with eating behaviours, lifestyle practices, personality characteristics, and menstrual irregularity in college women. Appetite 2003; 40: 185-192.*
151. McNamara K, Loveman C. Differences in family functioning among bulimics, repeat dieters and non-dieters. J Clin Psychol 1990; 46: 518-523.*
152. Mendelson B, McLaren L, Gauvin L, Steiger, H. The relationship of self-esteem and body esteem in women with and without eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 31: 318-323.*
153. Meyer C, Leung N, Feary R, Mann B. Core beliefs and bulimic symptomatology in non-eating disordered women; the mediating role of borderline characteristics. Int J Eat Disord 2001; 30: 434-440.*
154. Meyer D. Co-dependency as a mediator between stressful events and eating disorders. J Clin Psychol 1997; 53: 107-116.*
155. Miller D, McCluskey-Fawcett K. The relationship between childhood sexual abuse and subsequent onset of Bulimia nervosa. Child Abuse Negl 1993; 17: 305-314.*
156. Miller S, Redlich A, Steiner H. The stress response in anorexia nervosa. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2003; 33: 295-306.*
157. Milligan R, Waller G. Anger and bulimic psychopathology among non-clinical women. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 28: 450.*
158. Mintz L, Kashubeck S. Body image and disordered eating among Asian American and Caucasian college students: an examination of race and gender differences. Psychol Women Q 1999; 23: 781-796.*
159. Mizes S. Personality characteristics of bulimic and non-eating disordered female controls: a cognitive behavioural perspective. Int J Eat Disord 1988; 7: 541-550.*
160. Moller, A, Bothma M. Body dissatisfaction and irrational beliefs. Psychol Rep 2001; 88: 423-430.*
161. Mujtaba T, Furnham, A. A cross-cultural study of parental conflict and eating disorders in a non-clinical sample. Int J Soc Psychiatry 2001; 47: 24-35.*
162. Mumford D, Choudry I. Body dissatisfaction and eating attitudes in slimming and fitness gyms in London and Lahore; A cross-cultural study. Euro Eat Disord Rev 2000; 8: 217-224.*
163. Mumford D, Whitehouse A. Increased prevalence of bulimia nervosa among Asian schoolgirls. BMJ 1988; 297: 718.*
164. Mumford D, Whitehouse A, Platts, M. Socio-cultural correlates of eating disorders among Asian schoolgirls in Bradford. Br J Psychiatry 1991; 158: 222-228.*
165. Murray C, Waller G, Legg C. Family dysfunction and bulimic psychopathology: The mediating role of shame. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 28: 84-89.*
166. Nagata T, Matsuyama M, Kiriike N, Iketani T, Ohshima J. Stress coping strategy in Japanese patients with eating disorders: relationship with Bulimic and impulsive behaviours. J Nerv Ment Dis 2000; 188: 280-286.*
167. Narduzzi K, Jackson T. Personality differences between eating-disordered women and a non-clinical comparison sample: A discriminant classification analysis. J Clin Psychol 2000; 56: 699-710.*
168. Neff L, Sargent R, McKeown R, Jackson K, Valois R. Black-White differences in body size perceptions and weight management practices among adolescent females. J Adolesc Health 1997; 20: 459-465.*
169. Orzolek-Kronner C. The effect of attachment theory in the development of eating disorders: can symptoms be proximity-seeking. Child Adolesc Social Work J 2002; 19: 421-435.*
170. Parker R, Lambert M, Burlingame G. Psychological features of female runners presenting with pathological weight control behaviours. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1994; 16: 119-134.*
171. Pasman L, Thompson J. Body image and eating disturbance in obligatory runners, obligatory weightlifters and sedentary individuals. Int J Eat Disord 1988; 7: 759-769.*
172. Penas-Lledo E, Sancho L, Waller G. Eating attitudes and the use of alcohol, tobacco and exercise among male and female adolescents. Eat Behav 2002; 3: 101-111.*
173. Perry J, Silvera D, Rosenvinge J, Holte A. Are oral, obsessive and hysterical personality traits related to disturbed eating patterns? A general population study of 6,313 men and women. J Pers Assess 2002; 78: 405-416.*
174. Phillips L, Tiggemann M, Wade T. Comparison of cognitive style in bulimia nervosa and depression. Beh Res Ther 1997; 35: 939-948.*
175. Pidcock B, Fischer J, Forthun L, West S. Hispanic and Anglo college women's risk factors for substance use and eating disorders. Addict Behav 2000; 25: 705-723.*
176. Piggott T, Altemus M, Rubenstein C, Hill J, Bihari K, L'Heureux F, Bernstein S, Murphy D. Symptoms of eating disorders in patients with obsessive-Compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 1991; 148: 1552-1557.*
177. Pike K, Rodin J. Mothers, daughters and disordered eating. J Abnorm Psychol 1991; 100: 198-204.*
178. Pinkston M, Martz D, Domer F, Bazzini D, Smith L, Curtin L, Henson D. Psychological, nutritional, and energy expenditure differences in college females with anorexia nervosa versus comparable-mass controls. Eat Behav 2001; 2: 169-181.*
179. Pollice C, Kaye W, Greeno C, Weltzin T. Relationship of depression, anxiety, and obsessionality to state of illness in anorexia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 21: 367-376.*
180. Poulakis Z, Wertheim E. Relationships among dysfunctional cognitions, depressive symptoms and bulimic tendencies. Cog Ther Res 1993; 17: 549-559.*
181. Powell A, Kahn A. Racial differences in women's desires to be thin. Int J Eat Disord 1995; 17: 191-195.*
182. Rhea D. Eating disorder behaviours of ethnically diverse urban female adolescent athletes and non-athletes. J Adolesc 1999; 22: 379-388.*
183. Rhodes B, Kroger J. Parental bonding and separation-individuation difficulties among late adolescent eating disordered women. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 1992; 22: 249-263.*
184. Rogers R, Petrie T. Psychological correlates of anorexic and bulimic symptomatology. J Couns Dev 2001; 79: 178-186.*
185. Rorty M, Yager J, Rossotto E. Aspects of Childhood physical punishment and family environment correlates in bulimia nervosa. Child Abuse Negl 1995; 19: 659-667.*
186. Rorty M, Yager J, Rossotto E. Childhood sexual, physical and psychological abuse in bulimia nervosa. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 1122-1126.*
187. Rosch D, Crowther J, Graham J. MMPI-derived personality description and personality subtypes in an undergraduate bulimic population. Psychol Addict Behav 1991; 5: 15-22.*
188. Rowa K, Kerig P, Geller J. The family and anorexia nervosa: examining parent- child boundary problems. Euro Eat Disord Rev 2001; 9: 97-114.*
189. Rubenstein C, Altemus M, Pigott T, Hess A, Murphy D. Symptom overlap between OCD and BN. J Anx Disord 1995; 9: 1-9.*
190. Rucker C, Cash T. Body images, body size perceptions, and eating behaviours among African-American and white college women. Int J Eat Disord 1992; 12: 291-299.*
191. Ruderman A.J, Grace P.S. Bulimics and restrained eaters: A personality comparison. Addict Behav 1988; 13: 359-368.*
192. Ruggiero G, Levi D, Ciuna A, Sassaroli S. Stress situation reveals an association between perfectionism and drive for thinness. Int J Eat Disord 2003; 34: 220-226.*
193. Ryujin D, Breaux C, Marks A. Symptoms of eating disorders among female distance runners: can the inconsistencies be unraveled? Women Health 1999; 30: 71-83.*
194. Sanftner JL, Crowther JH. Variability in self-esteem, moods, shame, and guilt in women who binge. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 23: 391-397.*
195. Sansone R. A comparison of borderline personality symptomatology and self-destructive behavior in women with eating, substance abuse, and both eating and substance abuse disorders. J Personal Disord 1994; 8: 219-228.*
196. Schmidt U, Tiller J, Treasure J. Setting the scene for eating disorders: childhood care, classification and course of illness. Psychol Med 1993; 23: 663-672.*
197. Schneider JA, O'Leary A, Jenkins SR. Gender, sexual orientation, and disordered eating. Psychol Health 1995; 10: 113-128.*
198. Sharpe T, Ryst E, Hinshaw S, Steiner H. Reports of stress; A comparison between eating disordered and non-eating disordered adolescents. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 1997; 28: 117-132.*
199. Shaye R. Relationships between impulsivity and eating behaviour under varying conditions of stress and food deprivation. Pers Individ Dif 1989; 10: 805-808. *
200. Shisslak C, McKeon R, Crago M. Family dysfunction in normal weight bulimic and bulimic anorexic families. J Clin Psychol 1990; 46: 185-189.*
201. Showers C, Larson B. Looking at body image: the organisation of self-knowledge about physical appearance and its relation to disordered eating. J Pers 1999; 67: 659-700.*
202. Siever M. Sexual orientation and gender as factors in socio-culturally acquired vulnerability to body dissatisfaction and eating disorders. J Consult Clin Psychol 1994; 62: 252-260.*
203. Smith MC. Thelen MH. Development and Validation of a test for bulimia. J Consult Clin Psychol 1984; 52: 863-872.
204. Smolak L, Levine M, Sullins E. Are child sexual experiences related to eating-disordered attitudes and behaviours in a college sample? Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 167-178.*
205. Soukup V, Beiler M, Terrell F. Stress, coping style, and problem solving ability among eating disordered inpatients. J Clin Psychol 1990; 46: 592-599.*
206. Steiger H, Fraenkel L, Leichner P. Relationship of body-image distortion to sex-role identifications, irrational cognitions, and body weight in eating-disordered females. J Clin Psychol 1989; 45: 61-65.*
207. Steiger H, Jabalpurwala S, Champagne J, Stotland S. A controlled study of trait narcissism in anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1997; 22: 173-178.*
208. Steiger H, Liquornik K, Chapman J, Hussain N. Personality and family disturbances in eating disorder patients: comparison of restrictors and bingers to normal controls. Int J Eat Disord 1991; 10: 501-512.*
209. Steiger H, Stotland S, Ghadirian A, Whitehead V. Controlled study of eating concerns and psychopathological traits in relatives of eating-disordered probands: Do familial traits exist? Int J Eat Disord 1995; 18: 107-118.*
210. Steiger H, Van der Feen J, Goldstein C, Leichner P. Defense styles and parental bonding in eating disordered women. Int J Eat Disord 1989; 8: 131-140.*
211. Steinberg B, Shaw R. Bulimia as a disturbance of narcissism: self-esteem and the capacity to self-soothe. Addict Behav 1997; 22: 699-710.*
212. Steiner H. Defence styles in eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 149-161.*
213. Strauss J, Ryan R. Autonomy disturbances in subtypes of anorexia nervosa. J Abnorm Psychol 1987; 96: 254-258.*
214. Striegel-Moore R, Tucker N, Hsu J. Body image dissatisfaction and disordered eating in lesbian college students. Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 493-500.*
215. Strong S, Williamson D, Netemeyer R, Geer J. Eating disorder symptoms and concerns about body differ as a function of gender and sexual orientation. J Soc Clin Psychol 2000; 19: 240-255.*
216. Stuart G, Laraia M, Ballenger J, Lydiard R. Early family experiences of women with bulimia and depression. Arch Psychiatr Nurs 1990; 4: 43-52.*
217. Sungot-Borgen S, Corbin, C. Eating disorders among female athletes. Phys Sportsmed 1987; 15: 89-95.*
218. Suzuki K, Takeda A, Matsushita S. Co-prevalence of bulimia with alcohol abuse and smoking among Japanese male and female high school students. Addiction 1995; 90: 971-975.*
219. Szymanski L, Chrisler J. Eating disorders, gender-role and athletic activity. Psychol J Human Behav 1990; 27: 20-29.*
220. Tata P, Fox J, Cooper J. An investigation into the influence of gender and parenting styles on excessive exercise and disordered eating. Euro Eat Disord Rev 2001; 9: 194-206.*
221. Tchanturia K, Serpell L, Troop N, Treasure J. Perceptual illusions in eating disorder: rigid and fluctuating styles. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 2001; 32: 107-115.*
222. Tiggemann M, Pickering A. Role of television in adolescent women's body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness. Int J Eat Disord 1996; 20: 199-203.*
223. Tiggemann M, Ruutel E. A cross-cultural comparison of body dissatisfaction in Estonian and Australian young adults and it's relationship with media exposure. J Cross Cult Psychol 2001; 32: 736-742.*
224. Tomori M, Rus-Makovec M. Eating behaviour, depression, and self-esteem in high school students. J Adolesc Health 2000; 26: 361-367.*
225. Tripp M, Petrie T. Sexual abuse and eating disorder: A test of a conceptual model. Sex Roles 2001; 44: 17-32.*
226. Tsai G, Curbow B, Heinberg L. Socio-cultural and developmental influences on body dissatisfaction and disordered eating attitudes and behaviours of Asian women. J Nerv Ment Dis 2003; 191: 309-318.*
227. Twamley E, Davis M. The socio-cultural model of eating disturbance in young women; the effects of personal attributes and family environment. J Sport Exerc Psychol 1999; 18: 467-489.*
228. von Ranson K, Iacono W, McGue M. Disordered eating and substance use in an epidemiological sample: Associations within individuals. Int J Eat Disord 2002; 31: 389-403.*
229. Wagner N.J. Sense of personal ineffectiveness in patients with eating disorders: One construct or several. Int J Eat Disord 1987; 6: 495-505*
230. Waller G, Babbs M, Milligan R, Meyer C, OHanian V, Leung N. Anger and core beliefs in the eating disorders. Int J Eat Disord 2003; 34: 118-124.*
231. Waller G, Matoba M. Emotional eating and eating psychopathology in non-clinical groups: a cross-cultural comparison of women in Japan and the UK. Int J Eat Disord 1999; 26: 333-340.*
232. Waller G, O'Hanian V, Meyer C, Osman S. Cognitive content among bulimic women; the role of core beliefs. Int J Eat Disord 2000; 28: 235-241.*
233. Waller G, Ruddock A. Information-processing correlates of reported sexual abuse in eating-disordered and comparison women. Child Abuse Negl 1995; 19: 745-759.*
234. Waller G, Slade P, Calam R. Family adaptability and cohesion: relation to eating attitudes and disorders. Int J Eat Disord 1990b; 9: 225-228.*
235. Ward A, Ramsey R, Turnbull S, Steele M, Steele H, Treasure J. Attachment in anorexia nervosa; a transgenerational perspective. Br J Med Psychol 2001; 74: 497-505.*
236. Wardle J, Bindra R, Fairclough B, Westcombe A. Culture and body image; body perception and weight concern in young Asian and Caucasian British women. J Comm Appl Soc Psychol 1993; 3: 173-181.*
237. Warren B, Stanton A, Blessing D. Disordered eating patterns in competitive female athletes. Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 565-569.*
238. Watkins P, Cartiglia M, Champion J. Are type A tendencies in women associated with eating disorder pathology. J Gender Cult Health 1998; 3: 101-109.*
239. Welch S. Smoking and bulimia nervosa. Int J Eat Disord 1998; 23: 433-437.*
240. Welch S, Doll H, Fairburn C. Life events and the onset of bulimia nervosa: A controlled study. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 515-522.*
241. Welch S, Fairburn C. Impulsivity or co-morbidity in bulimia nervosa: A controlled study of deliberate self-harm and alcohol and drug misuse in a community sample. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 169: 451-458.*
242. Welch S, Fairburn C. Sexual abuse and bulimia nervosa: three integrated case control comparisons. Am J Psychiatry 1994; 151: 407.*
243. Wilfley D, Schreiber G, Pike K, Striegel-Moore R, Wright D, Rodin J. Eating disturbance and body image: A comparison of a community sample of adult Black and White women. Int J Eat Disord 1996; 20: 377-387.*
244. Willcox M, Sattler D. The relationship between eating disorders and depression. J Soc Psychol 1996; 136: 269-271.*
245. Wonderlich S, Crosby R, Mitchell J, Thompson K, Redlin J, Demuth G, Smyth J, Haseltine B. Eating disturbance and sexual trauma in childhood and adulthood. Int J Eat Disord 2001; 30: 401-412.*
246. Wonderlich S, Donaldson M, Carson D, Staton D, Gretz L, Leach L, Johnson M. Eating disturbance and incest. J Interpers Violence 1996; 11: 195-207.*
247. Xinaris S, Boland F.J. Disordered eating in relation to tobacco use, alcohol consumption, self control and sex-role ideology. Int J Eat Disord 1990; 9: 425-433.*
248. Young E, McFatter R, Clopton J. Family functioning, peer influence, and media influence as predictors of bulimic behaviour. Eat Behav 2001; 2: 323-337.*
249. Zaitsoff S, Geller J, Srikameswaran S. Silencing the self and suppressed anger: Relationship to eating disorder symptoms in adolescent females. Euro Eat Disord Rev 2002; 10: 51-60.*
250. Zlotnick C, Hohlstein L, Shea M, Pearlstein T, Recupero P, Bidadi K. The relationship between sexual abuse and eating pathology. Int J Eat Disord 1996; 20: 129-134.*

Author Information

Emma P. Vince, PhD
Department of Primary Care and General Practice, University of Birmingham

Ian Walker, PhD
Department of Psychology, University of Bath

Download PDF

Your free access to ISPUB is funded by the following advertisements:

 

BACK TO TOP
  • Facebook
  • Google Plus

© 2013 Internet Scientific Publications, LLC. All rights reserved.    UBM Medica Network Privacy Policy