Illumination of Shadowing Behavior in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease: Proximity-Seeking? As Life Becomes the “Strange Situation”
M Lilly, M Hermanns, J Beckstrand, A Booth, M Farlow
Citation
M Lilly, M Hermanns, J Beckstrand, A Booth, M Farlow. Illumination of Shadowing Behavior in Individuals with Alzheimer’s Disease: Proximity-Seeking? As Life Becomes the “Strange Situation”. The Internet Journal of Neurology. 2010 Volume 13 Number 2.
Abstract
The purpose of this paper is to suggest an
Introduction
Certain behaviors exhibited by an individual with Alzheimer’s disease can be particularly difficult for a caregiver to tolerate, including repetitive questioning, hiding objects, and shadowing. Shadowing, a common behavioral symptom in Alzheimer’s disease, is associated with increased caregiver stress (Cummings, 2005; Fauth, Zarit, Femia, Hofer, & Stephens, 2006; Gitlin, Winter, Burke, Chernett, Dennia, & Hauck, 2008; Kasl-Godley & Gatz, 2000; Nichols, Martindale-Adams, Greene, Burns, Graney, & Lummus, 2009). Shadowing refers to the close following of another individual (Brodaty, Draper, Low, 2003); and in the dementia caregiving situation, this usually means a spouse, adult child, other close family member or friend.
Specifying the incidence and prevalence of shadowing behavior within the family caregiving setting is problematic, presumably due to grouping and reporting of shadowing information within categories such as the Anxiety Subscale of the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings, Mega, Gray, Rosenberg-Thompson, Carusi, & Gornbein, 1994), or the Revised Memory and Behavior Problems Checklist (Teri, Truax, Logsdon, Uomoto, Zarit, Vitaliano, 1992), or possibly through failure to capture “other” response categories added to existing measures (Gitlin, Winter, Burke, Chernett, Dennis & Hauck, 2008; Gitlin, Winter, Corcoran, Dennis, Schinfeld, & Hauck, 2003). However, significant dementia stage-related differences in caregivers requests for behavioral management information were identified by Nichols, Martindale-Adams, Greene, Burns, Graney, and Lummus (2009) in their secondary analysis of data from a primary care-based psychoeducational intervention (Burns, Nichols, Martindale-Adams, Graney, & Lummus, 2003). The researchers reported that caregivers for individuals with mild dementia were more likely to request a pamphlet about shadowing (16.2%), as compared to caregivers for individuals with moderate or severe dementia (5.9%). During the primary care intervention study, behavior management pamphlets on various topics, including shadowing, were provided to caregivers depending problem behaviors identified by the caregiver. However, no definition or measure of shadowing was found in the primary intervention report or secondary analysis.
A robust proxy measure for shadowing is located on the Anxiety Subscale of The Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (Cummings, Mega, Gray, Rosenberg-Thompson, Carusi, & Gornbein, 1994), a widely implemented, standardized measure of non-cognitive symptoms in dementia. Specifically, the Anxiety Subscale of the NPI queries as to whether or not the care recipient exhibits fear of being away from the caregiver, or displays nervousness about being separated from the caregiver. Research has demonstrated an inverse relationship between Mini Mental Status Exam (MMSE) results (Porter, Buxton, Fairbanks, Strickland, O’Connor, Rosenberg-Thompson, & Cummings (2003) and Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) (Fuh, Wang, & Cummings (2005), and scores on the Anxiety Subscale of the NPI, indicating an increase in anxiety symptoms, including shadowing with declining cognitive function. Further, Steinberg, Shao, Zandi, Lyketsos, Welsh-Bohmer, Norton, Breitner, Steffens, Tschang, and the Cache County Investigators (2008) reported a point prevalence of NPI- assessed anxiety symptoms, including shadowing behaviors, of 14.1% at baseline with a 5-year cumulative prevalence of 62%. Anxiety symptoms were the third most prevalent in the Steinberg et al., study, following depression and apathy symptoms. Combined, these results provide useful, although imprecise information concerning shadowing behavior in individuals with Alzheimer’s disease or a related dementia.
Anecdotal caregiver comments gleaned from research and clinical settings enriches the description of what is known about shadowing behavior. Examples of anecdotal comments include, “if I leave the room,
While shadowing behavior may be understandable, given the care recipient’s increasing levels of dependency, it can be an interruption of other activities, and a hindrance to privacy, as the above examples and online postings illustrate (Alzheimer’s Association Online Community, 2011). On a deeper level, shadowing behavior is a persistent reminder of the illness and accompanying losses. The nearly constant shadowing or following by the care recipient, may be episodically difficult for caregivers, and for some a source of sustained emotional distress, which may manifest in many ways including anger. Acceptance and successful management of the behavior may be more or less difficult depending on a number of variables, including the history and ongoing nature of the caregiver and care recipient relationship, caregiver knowledge of the illness, caregiver depression, fatigue, or illness, and perceived social support (Alzheimer’s Association Online Community, 2011; Ingebretsen & Solem, 1998).
The purpose of this paper is to suggest an
Background
Clearly caregiving for a person with Alzheimer’s disease is a different experience from parenting a toddler; yet shadowing behavior has noticeable similarities to the proximity-seeking behavior seen in toddlers. Viewed from a caregiver response perspective, the “natural” behavioral pattern is positively biased toward the very young human; it being well-established that certain physical characteristics of the infant, toddler, and even preschooler make their following and calling out behaviors easier to accept and respond to in a caring manner (Kringelbach, Lehtonen, Squire, Harvey, Craske, Holliday, Green, Aziz, Hansen, Cornelissen, & Stein, 2008; Lishner, Oceja, Stocks, & Zaspel, 2008). In general, adults are not expecting to respond to these types of proximity-seeking behaviors in older adults, and this creates emotional distress for both individuals. At the same time, in our youth-oriented society, it is not uncommon for the mere appearance and behavior of old age to elicit fear, disgust, or at least ambivalence; all clearly avoidance and non-caring behaviors (McHugh, 2003; Nelson, 2005). However, for the dependent young or old, there is no doubt that the explanations for both caregiver and care recipient behaviors are complex, interacting, and related to survival.
Anger or frustration is a caregiver response behavior that is often elicited in the instance of shadowing by an elderly care recipient with Alzheimer’s disease or a related disorder (Ingebretsen & Solem, 1998). One can surmise that this response is due to a cognitive and emotional mindset different from the mindset operating in the typical parent-child attachment scenario; and therein rests an opportunity for both caregiver and care recipient. Mindset, as proposed by Bar (2009), refers to a state of cognitive activation resulting from exposure to an environmental cue that prompts associative activations related to the cue. Mindsets guide behavior through promoting the cognitive, behavioral, and action tendencies that are consistent with contextual requirements. In other words, mindset reduces the effort and attention needed for a particular set of environmental cues by providing a ‘top-down’ set of predictions based on prior experiences with similar cues, to inform the new experience. When the ‘bottom up’ information, meaning the information emerging from the new scenario fails to meet expectations, attention must be reallocated to answer the brain’s question, “what is this like?” (Bar, 2009). Fortunately, mindset can be altered as events unfold, as information is updated, and even by imagined experience such as simulations. It follows then, that outdated caregiver mindsets related to interactions with the care recipient can be updated and enhanced through various types of learning experiences. If caregivers for individuals with Alzheimer’s disease understand the neurological basis for the shadowing behavior, and are motivated to alter their own emotional and behavioral responses, it is possible that caregivers could select a more appropriate and comforting response to the person with dementia. It is likely that the caregivers more appropriate response will be reinforced by the care recipient’s positive behavior, and a positive feedback cycle will replace the previous negative cycle. Applying Bar’s (2009) notion of mindset to the caregiver for a person with Alzheimer’s disease, gives rise to the opportunity for a targeted intervention aimed at altering the caregiver’s care recipient related mindsets that are outdated by virtue of the disease progression. Eventual testing of this model will demonstrate whether or not knowledge combined with behavioral coaching and other intervention strategies can improve outcomes for caregivers and care recipients.
Data Sources
This manuscript was written using articles searched between January 2010 and February 2011 on Ovid Medline, PsychInfo and CINAHL databases. Inclusion criteria were for publications between 1973 to the present, written in the English language, and with a preference for articles with links to full text. Exclusion criteria were papers focused on formal or institutional caregiving, wandering, wayfinding, pacing, elopement, and getting lost. Key words included shadowing, following, Alzheimer’s disease, attachment, attachment theory, caregiving, orbitofrontal, orbitofrontal system, proximity-seeking, and mindset. Titles/abstracts were reviewed for relevance, with reading of the full text for those articles deemed relevant to the present discussion. Additional literature was accessed from references provided in articles deemed relevant.
Attachment Theory
Bowlby (1973) noted that human beings tend to be fearful in a number of situations including being alone or in the dark, being with strangers or strange animals, experiencing the rapid approach of others, and hearing loud noises. He further articulated that the fear of unwilling separation from one’s attachment figure, at any age or stage of life, is an instinctive response based on awareness of increased danger and risk to survival. Along similar lines, Klin, Jones, Schultz, and Volkman (2003) emphasized that the somatosensory apparatus of human beings continuously seeks salient aspects of the environment, particularly those aspects with survival value. It follows that the location and status of kin or caregivers would have a high degree of salience, as compared to competing information about non-kin or inanimate environmental objects.
Healthy attachment dynamics, which do not operate at a conscious level of awareness, reflect an interaction between “psychobiologically attuned organisms” (Schore, 2000, p. 34). In the prototypical infant-mother dyad this attunement allows for the rapid transmission of prosodic-auditory, visual, and gestural information included in the exchange between the two faces and beings, and in this exchange the caregiver serves as a regulator of the infant’s arousal level. As a regulator of arousal and soother of stressful negative emotion, the caregiver affects the direction of energy consumption, determining where valuable energy will be expended in the biosynthetic process. In a similar manner, according to Bowlby (1973), each person across her or his lifespan continues to benefit when life is organized around the secure base of an attachment figure; even though the appearance of the bond, and reciprocity expectations vary significantly from those observed between mother and infant. In fact, from toddlerhood forward, the maturing child and eventual adult, is increasingly encouraged and expected to manage their anxiety and fear with internal representations of mother and home, as well as institutional substitutes like teachers, and later romantic partners and platonic friends for several hours a day, and longer. In all but extreme cases, i.e., war trauma where there is threat to life or bodily integrity, vocal calls for mother or any attachment figure, would be considered highly unusual in mature, healthy adults.
A Model of Attachment
Mikulincer and Shaver’s (2007) model of attachment system activation and functioning in adults assumes cognitively intact individuals, and integrates conceptualizations developed by Bowlby, Ainsworth, Main and others (see Figure 1). These earlier conceptualizations proposed a universally present attachment system, activated automatically in the presence of perceived threat, in order to ensure survival by first eliciting behaviors to attract the caregiver to the infant’s side; and once the infant achieved mobility, causing the infant to seek proximity to the caregiver. Mikulincer and Shaver’s model is characterized by three components that capture the basic points contained in the earlier conceptualizations. Each component involves monitoring and appraisal by the individual perceiving threat; a process that may occur at an unconscious level. Monitoring and appraisal of threatening events occurs initially, followed by monitoring and appraisal of availability and responsiveness of the attachment figure; and finally, monitoring and appraisal of the utility of proximity-seeking as a coping strategy occurs (see Figure 1). The model allows for seeking proximity to an internalized attachment figure. Further the researchers note that over time an individual’s feelings of security can be enhanced or eroded by experience and learning, through engagement with caregivers or attachment figures of varying degrees of responsiveness and availability. Erosion of security by various means can lead to additional behaviors, like hyperactivation or deactivation of proximity-seeking, based on utility beliefs and prior reinforcement.
Due to the effects of neurodegeneration on memory and emotion, Mikulincer and Shaver’s model is modified, simplified in the sense of eliminating feedback, and clarified as to the proposed relationship to individuals with Alzheimer’s disease (see hatchmarks and notes to Figure 1). For these individuals,
I did my best to spend every moment with my mom because I knew our moments together would end too soon. As my mother’s Alzheimer’s progressed, I noticed her need to be with me increased. By the time she entered the middle stage, we saw the need for my family and I to move in with my parents. My dad still went to the office everyday and my sister Linda would come over and spend everyday with mom and I. It may not take a village to care for a loved one battling Alzheimer’s, but it does take a small devoted core of people who really care.
We started everyday with our coffee and sourdough bread toast, a daily ritual of Mother’s for well over twenty years. She loved to laugh, so we would then watch a comedy on the Lifetime for Women network as background to this morning regimen. Soon after, we would get dressed and I would help her with her jewelry and makeup. I believe that she not only loved the intimacy and attention, but that it was also reassuring for her to feel she was preparing herself for something. After all, as the consummate Southern Lady, Mother never wanted to be seen “undone.”
Next we would return to our chairs and another of her favorite TV shows, while she “read the paper.” The ability to read had long gone from her life, but holding on to the routines of life reassured her. She dwelled on every page, occasionally sharing a picture that caught her eye.
I would get up and excuse myself to get dressed and she would smile and nod in agreement. So I walked into my closet to choose my attire for the day, I would hear her entering my room, looking for me. She would offer an excuse for following me or ask a question. Of course she didn’t need an excuse or question. I could see the look of fear on her face. She wanted to be there with me and I wanted to have her close.
That need for reassurance drove Mother to take every step I did. She followed me everywhere. As hard as it is to watch your loved one decline so rapidly, that was trumped by the need to keep her feeling safe. Now that Mother is no longer with us, I find myself reliving & cherishing many of our moments together. Hearing her footsteps following me or just one more silly question or excuse would be a precious gift.
Now, as I go about my life, I catch my heart shadowing my mother, trying to walk just a few more steps with her, wanting to watch just one more silly sitcom. Since I spend time coaching other newer caregivers, shadowing is something that we talk about often. I try to tell these new caregivers to see the joy in it, to understand that something as simple as allowing a loved one to follow your steps can be more precious than gold or fine jewelry. It’s a good thing, a gift that will live in your memory a long long time.
Usually the primary caregiver is a spouse or adult child when a person is affected with Alzheimer’s disease or a related disorder. Much like an infant or toddler noting the absence of mother, when the person with Alzheimer’s disease or a related disorder misses their caregiver, that stimulus alone is sufficient to activate the attachment system. Typically, verbal queries will begin shortly after the caregiver is noted to be missing, restlessness will ensue, and locomotion will follow as the individual attempts to find their caregiver. However, individuals with Alzheimer’s disease and related disorders are unlikely to make use of an
Brain-Based Changes In Alzheimer’s Disease
the orbitofrontal system is also deeply connected into the autonomic nervous system and the arousal-generating reticular formation, . . . The activity of this frontolimbic system is therefore critical to the modulation of social and emotional behaviors and the homeostatic regulation of body and motivational states, affect-regulating functions that are centrally involved in attachment processes.
In early Alzheimer’s disease the orbitofrontal cortex is less affected by pathological changes as compared to the temporal lobe and hippocampus (Frisoni, Fox, Jack, Scheltens, & Thompson, 2010; Shiino, Watanabe, Maeda, Kotani, Akiguchi, & Matsuda, 2006; Zakzanis, Graham, & Campbell, 2003). The orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is located in the ventral section of the frontal lobe. This area integrates reward and sensory information through connections with the limbic system (hippocampus, amygdala, cingulate) and various sensory elements. Significantly, Lemche et al., (2006) reported bilateral amygdalar activation to a subliminally presented negative attachment stress prime, in their study of 12 health young adults. The researchers further stated that significant positive correlations were found between the magnitude of amygdalar activation, and level of attachment insecurity and autonomic activation. Lemche et al., encouraged exploration of the linkage of their findings with animal and human studies on the role of oxytocin in maternal care, and the abundance of oxytocin receptors in amagdaloid tissue.
Motor and autonomic function are also influenced through OFC connections with other structures including the nucleus accumbens, hypothalamus, and periaqueductal gray matter (Wallis, 2007). The anterior insular cortex (AIC), a structure bordering the OFC, may have an important role in individual maintenance of both self and other awareness (Craig, 2009), and is therefore of interest to the current discussion. Kringlebach (2005) referred to the OFC as a “gateway to subjective conscious experience” (p. 699), with an important role in assessing the emotional valence of environmental events.
The information presented in this section allows for the possibility that survival-based attachment behaviors like proximity-seeking, may originate in the orbitofrontal cortex. As Alzheimer’s disease progresses, and even when the care recipient no longer recognizes a spouse or adult child as being related, it is possible that pre-conscious information about these primary attachment figures is activated by the still intact instinct for survival. The care recipient’s emphatic unwillingness to be separated from their primary caregiver is every bit as convincing as the young child clinging to their mother “as if for dear life”.
Discussion and Recommendations
Interventions for managing shadowing, or proximity-seeking, have met with varying success. Tested approaches include providing information (Nichols et al, 2009), using reassurance or a distraction (Brodaty, Draper, & Low, 2003), and implementing a planned, tailored activity (Gitlin et al., 2008). Gitlin et al., hypothesized that their community-focused, individualized, activity intervention reduced shadowing and other behaviors through building on preserved care recipient abilities, including matching to prior habits, roles, and interests. The researchers also provided teaching instruction and stress reduction techniques to caregivers with emphasis on modifying and simplifying prescribed activities in response to expected future declines. The researchers speculated that care recipients responded positively to the individually tailored intervention because the activities allowed for enhanced role identity, an important aspect of selfhood, and for the productive use of time. Central values demonstrated by the tailored intervention approach were to avoid care recipient overload through task and environmental simplification, and to engage the caregiver through education, training, support, and positive care recipient outcomes.
In a related approach, Buckwalter et al., (1999) and Smith, Gerdner, Hall, and Buckwalter (2004) implemented a psychoeducational intervention for caregivers through presentation of the Progressively Lowered Stress Threshold model, followed by individual assessment of caregiver reported care recipient behavioral symptoms, and development of tailored interventions based on concepts and prepositions flowing from the model. In the Buckwalter et al., intervention researchers demonstrated the effectiveness of teaching caregivers about disease-based changes in care recipients’ ability to tolerate stress, and the particular nature of the stressors, including fatigue. Further, the researchers identified care recipient behaviors likely to result from sustained or overwhelming exposure to stress, and proposed theory-based caregiver and environmental alterations needed to accommodate the disease-based changes.
An underlying assumption of interventions with a psychoeducational component, like the Tailored Activity Program TAP and the PLST training, is that caregivers will be motivated, and have the capacity, to learn and apply the concepts and prepositions within the model (Smith, Gerdner, Hall, & Buckwalter, 2004). This assumption rests in learning theory and related knowledge from the social sciences. Findings from the TPA and PLST interventions confirm the correctness of this underlying assumption, and also provide support for the development of additional interventions using theory and models to illustrate and discuss underlying processes about a given phenomenon. The principles are as follows: to the degree possible, researchers and clinicians should provide a research-based account of the phenomenon of interest, like shadowing, and link the account to “usual experience” to strengthen learning through association. Then, a detailed description of caregiver, care recipient and environmental conditions most likely to result in minimizing deviations from “usual” must be carefully scaffolded to the care recipient’s particular history and behavior symptoms. Finally, periodic reinforcement of model application through follow-up visits, telephone calls, groups meetings or televisits is needed to counteract a natural tendency to return to “standard operating procedures” (SOPs) for interacting with another human being. Importantly, these SOPs may vary from couple to couple, family to family, and culture to culture, but application of the principle is consistent.
Based on currently available information, the concepts and propositions of human attachment theory, combined with knowledge about the structure and function of the normal and degenerating brain, hold promise for the design of psychoeducational interventions aimed at equipping caregivers to interact effectively with care recipients who shadow. Specifically, such interventions would include a) basic information about the automatic nature of the threat response and survival behavior, b) general information about neurodegeneration, and the progressive loss of important brain-based associations, c) specific information about how loss of brain-based associations increases the difficulty of the care recipient in interpreting environmental stimuli, d) theory based information, through application of the proximity-seeking concept to shadowing behavior, and e) intersubjective information specifying how, for the care recipient, all places may seem like Ainsworth and Bell’s “strange situation”(Ainsworth & Bell, 1971). An aIdditional experiential component of such an intervention could illustrate the pervasiveness and inflexibility of human expectations for social behavior, as well as the difficulty in “over-riding” those expectations when interacting with an individual who has adult physical characteristics, even if they are functioning at a much lower level. Finally, caregivers with varying perspectives could serve as peer interveners, sharing stories and experiences, providing assistance in reframing the meaning of shadowing behavior, and exploring alternative emotional and behavioral responses to shadowing (see Table 1). Research is needed to examine the effects of peer interveners engaged in story sharing and problem solving with other caregivers, whether by face to face communication, books and other written materials, media like video and CDs, or social networking via blogs, online forums, and other online offerings.
Post intervention journaling is another activity with the potential to facilitate caregiver reflection and changes in mind-set. Through journaling about the details of the day-to-day passing of time in the company of an individual suffering from Alzheimer’s disease, the caregiver may arrive at important personal and interpersonal realizations. A Caregiver’s Story (see Table 1) also suggests that journaling could be a means of preserving personal, poignant, and humorous memories of times spent with a loved one, to be treasured, shared, consulted, and revisited over time.
Implications for Nursing and Other Disciplines
This paper contributes to the field of neurology by setting forth a plausible, theory-based rationale for shadowing behavior, by providing research-based examples of interventions for caregiver education, and by suggesting avenues for future research. Further research is needed to understand the overall complexity of the brain, as well as pathways and mechanisms underlying a specific behavior, such as shadowing. A combination of genetic, imaging, and observational data, best gathered in the context of an interdisciplinary research team, is likely to yield the richest results. Further depth would be achieved by capturing caregiver and care recipient context via unobtrusive video and GPS technology. Implementation and assessment of interventions would also benefit from technological infusion. At this moment, we are far from understanding simple behavioral symptoms and their impact on caregiver and care recipient well-being. However, employing the full complement of current resources, along with interdisciplinary synergy, could inform many of Bowlby’s unanswered questions and musings.